DOCUMENT RESUME
ED 422 562
CS 013 266
AUTHOR
Wilson, Margaret S.
TITLE
Skills-Based or Whole Language Reading Instruction? A
Comparative Study in the Improvement of the Reading
Comprehension of High School Students.
PUB DATE
1998-08-00
NOTE
72p.; M.A. Thesis, Salem-Teikyo University.
PUB TYPE
Dissertations/Theses
Masters Theses (042) -- Reports -
Research (143)
EDRS PRICE
MF01/PC03 Plus Postage.
DESCRIPTORS
Classroom Techniques; Comparative Analysis; Conventional
Instruction; *High School Students; High Schools;
*Instructional Effectiveness; Learning Strategies; *Reading
Comprehension; *Reading Improvement; Reading Research;
*Remedial Reading; *Whole Language Approach
IDENTIFIERS
Metropolitan Achievement Tests; West Virginia
ABSTRACT
Practitioners and researchers argue about how reading should
be taught, about what students should read, and about how best to organize
reading instruction in the classroom. Diverse methods are used to teach
reading. These range from isolated skills-based (intensive phonics/basal
reading programs) to integrated, whole language approaches that stress
language experience, reading, writing, and critical thinking. Since a
sobering percentage of American children have difficulty in learning to read,
an inquiry was made into how best to reverse this trend in a high school
remedial reading classroom. The study's purpose was to compare the teaching
of an integrated, whole language approach to remedial reading using the novel
to a traditional, skills-based approach. The area of interest was reading
comprehension. Subjects were 54 students enrolled in high school remedial
reading classes at North Marion High School in north central West Virginia.
The measuring device was the Metropolitan Achievement Test. Data were
obtained from a control group and an experimental group. Analysis of the data
indicated that an experimental group of high school remedial reading students
who received whole language instruction using the novel and a control group
that received traditional, skills-based instruction scored equally on a
standardized reading comprehension test. Since no significant difference was
shown in the two methods of reading instruction, the conclusion is that
individualization and a balance between whole language and skills-based
instruction is most beneficial to high school remedial reading students.
(Contains 6 tables of data and 65 references.)
(CR)
********************************************************************************
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made
from the original document.
********************************************************************************
.r
1
SKILLS-BASED OR WHOLE LANGUAGE READING
INSTRUCTION?
A COMPARATIW STUDY IN
THE IMPROVEMENT OF THE READING COMPREHENSION
OF RIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS
A Thesis
Presented to
The Faculty of the Master of Arts Degree Program
Salem-Teikyo University
In Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree
Master of Arts in Education
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS
BEEN GRANTED BY
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
By
Margaret S. Wilson
August 1998
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
Office of Educational Research and Improvement
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER (ERIC)
tie(115
document has been reproduced as
received from the person or organization
originating it,
0 Minor changes have been made to improve
reproduction Quality.
Points of view or opinions stated in this docu-
ment do not necessarily represent official
.
OERI position or policy.
Salem-Teiyko University
Salem, West Virginia
This thesis submitted by Margaret S. Wilson has been
approved meeting the
research requirements for the Master of Arts Degree.
gticlz
Thesis Committee Chair
van der
ssen, Ph.D., Associate Professor, Salem-
ikyo Univ
ity, Salem, West Virginia
Committee Member
Ben Guido, Ed.D., Adjunct Professor of Education, Salem-
Teiyko University, Salem, West Virginia
p.
Gary S icAllister, Professor of Education, Salem-Teiyko
qu_
University, Salem, West Virginia
SKILLS-BASED OR WHOLE LANGUAGE
READING INSTRUCTION?
A COMPARATIVE STUDY IN
THE IMPROVEMENT OF THE READING COMPREHENSION
OF HIGH SCHOOL REMEDIAL READING STUDENTS
ABSTRACT
Practitioners and researchers argue about how reading should be taught,
about what students should read, and about how best to organize reading
instruction in the classroom. Diverse methods are used to teach reading. These
range from isolated skills-based (intensive phonics/basal reading programs) to
integrated, whole language approaches that stress language experience, reading,
writing, and critical thinking. Since a sobering percentage of American children
have difficulty learning to read, this inquiry was made into how best to reverse this
trend in a high school remedial reading classroom.
The purpose of this study was to compare the teaching of an integrated,
whole language approach to remedial reading using the novel to a traditional,
skills-based approach. The area of interest was reading comprehension. The
study involved 54 students enrolled in high school remedial reading classes at
North Marion High School in north central West Virginia The measuring device
in the study was the Metropolitan Achievement Test.
The data in this study were obtained from a control group and an
experimental group. The analysis of the data indicated that an experimental group
of high school remedial reading students who received whole language instruction
1
using the novel and
a control group that received traditional, skills-based
instruction scored equally
on a standardized reading comprehension test. Since
no
significant difference
was shown in the two methods of reading instruction in this
study, the conclusion is that individualization and
a balance between whole
language and skills-based instruction is
most beneficial to high school remedial
reading students.
II
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT
Chapter 1
THE PROBLEM AND ITS SETTING
1
Introduction
1
Purpose of Study
2
Research Question
2
Hypothesis
2
Definition of Terms
3
Assumptions
6
Limitations
6
Importance of the Study
7
Chapter 2
REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE
9
Introduction
9
The Ability to Read
9
Skills-Based Instruction
17
Whole Language
22
in
A Balanced Approach to Reading Comprehension
28
Conclusion
34
Chapter 3
METHOD
36
Subjects
36
Design
37
Procedure
38
Instrumentation
40
Chapter 4
ANALYSIS OF DATA
41
Statement of the Problem
41
Subjects
41
Homogeneity
44
The Hypothesis
45
Findings and Interpretations
45
Chapter 5
SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS
48
Summary
48
iv
Conclusions
49
Recommendations
50
REFERENCES
52
APPENDICES
A. Control Group Data
58
B. Experimental Group Data
59
C. Gender Graphs
60
D. Grade Graphs
61
E. Age Graphs
62
F. Pretest Scores
63
G. Post-test Scores
64
TABLES
1. Gender of Students in Sample
42
2. Grade Level of Students in Sample
43
3. Age of Students in Sample
43
4. Pretest Scores - Reading Comprehension
44
5. Post-test Scores
- Reading Comprehension
46
6. Comparison of Pretest and Post-test Scores
47
8
Chapter 1
THE PROBLEM AND ITS SETTING
Introduction
The main goal of education is to teach people to read. Reading is one of
the principal ways of transmitting a culture from one generation to another; failure
to learn to read can keep people from full participation in society (Wolf, 1993).
Reading is a complex skill. Although the exact details are not known, the
process begins when the reader looks at printed material, perceives the
organization of the letters, connects the visual perception and auditory sensation,
selects meaning given in the context, and finally comprehends the passage. In
order for these skills to take place reflexively when a child reaches maturity, the
school builds these skills in the child through study and practice. The main task of
reading instruction is to provide opportunities for working on these skills (Wolf,
1993).
Practitioners and researchers argue about how reading should be taught,
about what students should read, and about how best to organize reading
instniction in the classroom (Fay, 1956). Diverse methods are used to teach
reading. These range from isolated skills-based (intensive phonics/basal reading
programs) to integrated, whole language approaches that stress language
experience, reading, writing, and critical thinking. Since a sobering percentage of
1
9
American children have difficulty learning to read, this inquiry was made into how
best to reverse this trend in a high school remedial reading classroom (Kolstad &
Bardwell, 1997).
Purpose of Study
The purpose of this study was to compare two groups of students in a high
school remedial reading classroom. One group was instructed using the traditional
skills-based method and the other using an integrated whole language approach.
Standardized tests were administered prior to and immediately following the study
to determine if significant differences existed in the reading comprehension of the
two groups.
Research Question
The study was guided by the research question: Do high school remedial
reading students who receive whole language instruction using the novel score
higher on standardized reading comprehension tests than students instructed with a
traditional skills-based method?
Hypothesis
Ho: Students in a high school remedial reading classroom who receive
whole language instruction using the novel and students who receive traditional
2
skills-based method of instniction will
score equally on a standardized reading
comprehension test.
:
Students in a high school remedial reading classroom
who receive
whole language instruction using the novel will
score higher on a standardized
reading comprehension test than students who receive traditional
skills-based
method of instruction.
Balanced approach
- Using the balanced approach to teach reading, teachers blend
basic skills instruction with exposure to rich literature and writing
(Manzo, 1997).
Phonics is taught in systemic fashion, within the context of real stories
(Diegmueller, 1996).
Distlindinungiam
- The basal reading program is a complete package of
teaching materials, including student reader, teacher's manual, student workbook,
practice sheets, word cards, filmstrips, audio cassette tapes, and tests (Demos,
1987).
ft,citzin - Behaviorism is a theory or doctrine that studies human behavior
through analysis of objectively observable behavioral events, in contrast with
subjective mental states (Flexner, 1987).
Cognitive psychology
- Cognitive psychology is a branch studying the mental
processes involved in perception, learning, memory, and reasoning (Flexner,
3
1 1
1987).
Functionally illiterate
- A person who is functionally illiterate has some basic
education but still falls short of a minimum standard of literacy
or whose reading
and writing skills are inadequate for everyday needs (Flexner, 1987).
Intrinsic phonics - Intrinsic phonics is phonics that is taught more gradually, in the
context of meaningful reading (Chall, 1983).
Phoneme - In linguistics study, a phoneme is the smallest unit of speech that
distinguishes one word element from another in the English language (Esher,
1997).
Phonics
- Phonics is instruction in the relationship between letters and speech
sounds. The purpose of phonics is to teach students the alphabetic principle,
enabling them to see the relationship between letters and sounds and letters and
meanings, thus assisting them in the identification of known words and to
independently figure out unfamiliar words (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, &
Wilkinson, 1985).
Phonological awareness
- Phonological awareness is defmed as the ability to hear
and manipulate sound units in the language, such as syllables, the major parts of
syllables, and phonemes (Adams & Bruck, 1995).
Prior knowledge - Prior knowledge has to do with information gained in other
times and other places, knowledge that students potentially can bring with them to
their reading (Gilles, Bixby, Crowley, Crenshaw, Henrichs, Reynolds, & Pyle,
4
12
1988).
Reading
- Reading is the process of constructing meaning from written
text.
Reading is so complex that it requires
the coordination of a number of interrelated
skills (Anderson, et al., 1985).
Reading comprehensigi
- The researcher's definition of reading comprehension is
"the process of constructing meaning through
the dynamic interaction
among the
reader's existing knowledge, the information
suggested by the written language,
and the context of the reading situation" (Wixson
& Peters, 1984, p. 4).
Semantics
- Semantics is the study of meaningful and linguistic development by
examining changes in meaning and form (Flexner, 1987).
Skills-based instruction
- The traditional theory of learning to read is that children
learn a complex skill like reading by first making
sense of the smallest components
of the language (letters) and then progressing
to larger components (sounds,
words, and sentences). Children learn to read by decoding the
language
(Diegmueller, 1996).
Whokimmage Reading instruction that
can be called "whole language" teaches
children to create meaning from a transaction/interaction of information and the
reader's existing knowledge. Whole language involves
an interchange among
reader, text, environment, and the reading
process (Gutknecht, 1989). Whole
language means that all of language (reading, writing, listening, and speaking) in
an integrated form must be presented to students if they are to learn to read and
5
1
0
write (Gilles, et al., 1988).
Assumptions
1. The sample in this study
was adequate in size.
2. The sample in this study was typical of high school
remedial reading students.
3. The students in the whole language
group and the direct skills instniction group
were of equal intelligence.
4. The teaching methods used with the two
groups were significantly different.
5. The time frame of this study was adequate.
6. All of the students in this study
were present during the duration of this study.
7. The Metropolitan Achievement Test used in this study is valid.
8. The Metropolitan Achievement Test provides valid and adequate information
in
the evaluation of students' reading comprehension.
Limitations
1. This study was geographically limited to a rural high school community in
north central West Virginia.
2. The students in this study were largely from middle and lower socio-economic
families. The majority of the students were Caucasian.
3. The students in the classes involved in this study
were all below their grade
level in reading comprehension.
6
14
4. The students in this study
were in grades nine through twelve and
were
recommended for remedial reading instruction.
They were randomly placed in
these classes by a computer scheduling
process.
5. Some of the students in this study
received language instruction from other
language arts instructors. This additional
instruction may have influenced results.
6. One teacher instructed both the whole language
group and the traditional
instruction group.
7. The Metropolitan Achievement Test
was the sole testing device used for
comparison in this study.
8. Reading comprehension
was the only component of reading instruction that
was measured by this study.
Importance of' the Study
The traditional method of reading instruction began with reading lessons
that focused on phonics, emphasized
a tightly controlled vocabulary, and
continued with short "basal" (basic) reading
passages, followed by "skill-and drill"
exercises, each with only one correct answer (Diegmueller, 1996).
The introduction of whole language into the reading classroom represented
a different philosophy about the teaching and learning of language in the
classroom. Whole language became an innovation that stressed that children
use
language in ways that relate to their own lives and cultures and that involve all the
7
15
communication skills
- reading, writing, listening, speaking (Diegmueller, 1996).
Much research has been done on these two powerful schools of thought.
Many school systems insist that the curriculum reflect a time-honored reliance
upon phonics and basal readers. Conversely, the whole language approach is
embraced by many educators. For a time the whole language approach
was SO
dominant, that when reading test scores remained stagnant, or
even declined, a
powerful pro-phonics backlash occurred. Many experts now favor a balance
between the two approaches, blending the best of both whole language and
traditional reading instruction (Diegmueller, 1996).
Since research is limited as to the preferred method of remediating high
school readers, this study is a comparison between the traditional, direct, skills-
based method of teaching reading and whole language instruction, using high
interest/low level novels. If it is determined that one method brings better results
in the reading comprehension of high school remedial reading students, a
recommendation to retain and expand that method would be warranted.
8
16
Chapter 2
REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE
Introduction
The purpose of this study was to
compare the teaching of an integrated
approach to remedial reading using the novel
to a more traditional, skills-based
approach to remedial reading. The review of the
literature includes the following
topics: the ability to read, skills-based instruction, whole
language, and a balanced
approach to reading comprehension.
The Abiliti to Read
In earlier days, the vast majority of people could neither read
nor write.
They received information by word of mouth. Stories about their families
or their
leaders were passed down from generation to generation of listeners. Only
the
most important records were kept in writing, and anyone who could read and write
was a member of a special ruling class. Today, the ability to read is necessary for
nearly everyone. Adults who cannot read or who
are functionally illiterate find it
difficult to get a job, find housing, buy food, or receive medical
care. Reading is
no longer a special skill; it is a basic requirement (Chall, Popp, & Hirshberg,
1993).
Primitive man made pictures, using pointed flintflake
on stone, bone, horn,
9
17
and every other conceivable material, to communicate ideas, and there is evidence
that the pictures were quite similar throughout the world. Later, the symbols came
to represent ideas, rather that just single words. In the course of time man has
developed many types of written languages and found that language consists of
sounds that are represented by written characters. The ability to understand these
characters became increasingly important. By the beginning of the 19th century,
the ability to read was so highly valued that increasing numbers of children
attended school (Wolf, 1993).
Thus, reading instruction became the major job of the school. School
success or failure can often be traced to the child's success or failure at learning to
read. It is estimated that 75 percent of what a student learns in high school is
learned through reading (Fay, 1956). All school subjects depend heavily on
reading. When a school improves its reading program, improvement can be seen
in the various subjects being aught in that school. Many high school students
become dropouts because they are unable to read on a level in keeping with the
demands of the subjects in which they are enrolled (Aaron, 1961).
Reading is a basic life skill and becomes the basis for a child's success in
school. Without the ability to read well, opportunities for personal fulfillment and
job success will inevitably be lost. Society and individuals place much importance
on reading. In the classrooms across the country, reading
is an essential tool for
success. Not being able to read textbooks, do research in the library, or even
read
10
18
the teacher's notes on the blackboard directly affects
the quality and quantity of
a
child's learning (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, &
Wilkinson, 1985).
Adult readers and even high school teachers
often forget how difficult it
might have been for
some children to learn to read and take for granted the
many
times reading ability is used. Reading touches all
aspects of life and, to a large
extent, influences one's lifestyle, often being the determining factor
in one's
employment opportunities. Thus, literacy issues have
become the concern, not
only of schools, but of society and legislators alike. Adult
literacy and
remediation classes aid people in becoming
more self-sufficient, thus easing both
human and political issues such
as unemployment and welfare costs.
Psychologists know that reading can help relieve depression and
boredom (Leu &
Kinzer, 1987).
Reading is a complex process, involving
an intermingling of at least the
following elements: people (readers and writers), language, and printed
matter. In
simplest terms reading is defined as "getting information from the printed page"
or
"communication between an author and a reader" (Smith & Johnson, 1980, p.201).
A few decades ago, Leonard Bloomfield (1942), a noted American linguist,
referred to reading as the greatest intellectual feat of anyone's lifetime
Reading is important for society as well as for the individual. While
a
country receives a good return on its investment in education at all levels, nursery
school through college, research shows that the returns are highest from the early
11
19
years of schooling when children are first learning to read
(Psacharopoulos, 1981).
The Commission on Excellence (1983) warned of
the risk for America from
shortcomings in secondary education. The early
years set the stage for later
learning. Without the ability to read well, excellence
in high school and beyond is
unattainable (Chall, 1983).
As knowledge of the reading process has evolved,
so have the definitions
of reading become more complex. Although "getting
meaning from print" is one
way to define reading, such simplified definitions do not adequately identify the
complexity of the process, nor do they reflect the interaction of factors
which enter
into the reading process. Rudolf Flesch (1981) relates reading
to a set of
mechanical skills. In his view, "Learning to read is like learning
to drive a car...the
child learns the mechanics of reading; and, when he is through, he
can read" (p.3).
Dechant (1982) feels that reading is more complex. He states that reading
cannot
occur unless the pupil can identify and recognize the printed symbol, and be able
to give it a name. Meaning is absolutely essential in reading. Dechant speculates
that too much emphasis in reading instniction has been placed on word
identification and not enough on comprehension. Rumelhart (1986)
sees even
more intricacies in the reading process. He states that:
Reading is the process of understanding written language. It begins with a
flutter of patterns on the retina and ends when successful with a definite
idea about the author's intended message,...a skilled reader must be able to
make use of sensory, syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic information to
accomplish his task. These various sources of information interact in many
12
2 0
complex ways during the
process of reading (p.722).
Another definition by Leu and Kinzer (1987)
states that "Reading is a
developmental, interactive, and global
process involving learned skills. The
process specifically incorporates an individual's linguistic knowledge
and can be
both positively and negatively influenced by
nonlinguistic internal and external
variables or factors" (p.9).
Defmitions of reading are personal, based
on one's view of how one reads
or on the method by which one has been taught to read. Any definition of reading
is only a guide and must change
as the knowledge of the reading process grows.
Reading is a complex skill requiring the coordination of
a number of interrelated
sources of information. Becoming a skilled reader is a matter of continuous
practice, development, and refinement. Learning to read ought
to be a delightful,
successful experience for children. Some students, however, find learning
to read
a drudgery, a difficult and frustrating task (Anderson, et al., 1985).
Reading serves many purposes. It is a means of gathering information, it
can provide relaxation and entertainment, as well as being a means to pleasure
through the learning of new ideas. Reading can be silent
or, on occasion, oral
(Stauffer, 1980). Being able to read does not make a person smart
or productive,
but being able to read makes it possible for a person to function more intelligently
and effectively within modern society. This is the perspective which teachers of
reading most need to develop (Baird, 1987). The final goal for teaching reading
13
21
must be to produce readers, not readers just
on grade level, but lovers of reading
and devourers of reading (Carbone, 1987).
One of the most agreed
upon conclusions of modem research about reading
deals with the complexity of the reading
process. Anderson et al. (1985) describes
this complexity as follows:
Based on what we now know, it is incorrect
to suppose that there is a
simple or single step which, if taken correctly, will immediately
allow a
child to read. Becoming a skilled reader is
a journey that involves many
steps. Similarly, it is unrealistic to anticipate that
some one critical feature
of instruction will be discovered which, if in place, will
assure rapid
progress in reading. Quality instruction involves many elements.
Strengthening any one element yields small gains. For large gains,
many
elements must be in place (p.4).
Further, the Commission on Reading states in its report, Becoming
a Nation
of Readers, "while there is more consensus about reading than in the
past, there
are still important issues about which reasonable people disagree" (Anderson, et
al., 1985, p.4). Practitioners and researchers still
argue about how reading should
be taught, about what students should read, and about how best to organize reading
instruction in classrooms. Specialists in the reading field make fervent claims for
their approaches to a particular aspect of reading, whether it be intensive phonics,
basal reader approach, reading/writing connection, whole language, or high-level
thinking skills as "the answer" for teachers of reading. The lack of a body of
agreed-upon knowledge in the field of reading means not only that the arguments
will last a long time, but that various kinds of exaggerated claims will continue to
14
22
be made. This lack of a foundation of knowledge has much
to do with the swings
and fads for which the reading field is known (Winograd &
Greenlee, 1986).
Recent revision of theories and instructional methods have been made
because of the tremendous amount of research that has been done in the
area of
reading in the 1970's, 1980's, and 1990's. Researchers speculate that changes in
theories and the competitive nature of reading instruction
may produce seemingly
able readers who only engage in reading for external rewards, such
as grades, and
who never develop a love of reading. Children who are placed in reading
groups
that are compared to each other come to view reading as a competitive activity.
School systems that allow reading goals to be determined by test
scores, textbook
companies, outside educators, and the public come to view reading as a
competition (Van Prooyen & Clouse, 1994). Researchers also argue that teachers
have become "deskilled" in the sense that they no longer exercise their
professional judgment in deciding what to teach, how to teach, and when it should
be taught (Shannon, 1983, 1984; Shulman, 1983; Woodward, 1986). Shannon
(1983) believes that teachers exercise instructional control only to the extent that
their textbooks will allow. Some of the blame can be put on the use of commercial
reading materials (i.e. basal readers and workbooks), while other blame can be
placed upon the politics of school systems, classroom management issues, and
accountability. Teachers differ widely in their reading instruction goals (Calfee &
Drum, 1979). Calfee and Drum (1979) speculate that when a teacher is ineffective
15
2 3
in helping students achieve reading goals, that perhaps changing
that teacher's
goals would be an essential step in changing practices. They
suggest more intense
teacher preservice and inservice in order to provide
more coherent reading
instruction. Some teachers may be spending too much time guiding
children
through materials by assigning them activities and worksheets
and too little time
engaged in the kind of teaching that will help children appreciate
what they are
reading and develop into independent readers (Winograd & Greenlee,
1986).
Freedom for teachers to make curriculum judgments is essential
to the
development of a quality reading
program. Classroom teachers know more about
their individual students than do the authors and publishers of reading
programs.
It is critical that teachers use their knowledge in selecting and adapting materials
and lessons that suit the needs, strengths, and interests of their students. Teachers
must be enthusiastic in order to create quality reading program. They must share
their love of reading with children (Van Prooyen & Clouse, 1994).
lf, indeed, teachers are expected to teach children to read and to help them
improve, refine, and apply their reading abilities, then the teacher and the school
district must decide on a sensible approach to the teaching of reading. If, indeed,
one of the major objectives of the school is to help each student become an
independent reader, then the classroom organization used must help students learn
and gain proficiency in applying reading skills to the reading of all types of
material (Van Prooyen & Clouse, 1994).
16
2 9
Skills-Based Instruction
The traditional theory of learning
to read, which became part of the
instructional process with the beginning of
mass schooling in the 19th century, is
that children learn a complex skill like reading by first
making sense of the
smallest components of the language (letters) and then
progressing to larger
components (sounds, words, and sentences). When reading instruction
occurs
through skills-based or direct instruction, children learn
to read by learning to
decode the language, and understanding follows after they break the
code and
master the components. Traditional American education begins, therefore, with
reading lessons that focus on phonics (sounding out first letters, then combinations
of letters), tightly controlled vocabulary, and short "basal" (or basic) reading
passages, followed by numerous skills exercises, each with only one correct
answer (Diegmueller, 1996).
Traditional reading instruction involves the teaching of specific, isolated,
and often abstract skills. Teachers check off one skill after another until all
are
taught and learned, and then, at least from a basic skills perspective, the child is
a
competent reader. Reading instructional materials and programs are developed
based upon detailed, highly sequential, step-by-step manuals of directions for
teachers (Gutknecht, 1989).
In linguistics study, the smallest unit of speech that distinguishes one word
element from another in the English language is called a phoneme. Every
17
2 5
language consists of a specific group of phonemes. English is made up of 45
distinct sounds, 25 of which are consonant sounds and 19 which are vowel sounds.
As with all alphabetic languages, the emphasis of teaching reading using phonics
has been on correlating the written print to the phonetic sounds used in the spoken
language. For years, this method of teaching reading and writing did not vary
much. Children are taught, going from the simple to the complex: first, the
alphabet, then simple short syllables and then words. Once their decoding skills
have developed to a reasonable level, children are introduced to short "basal"
reading passages, stories, and poems. Children are expected to use their phonetic
decoding skills whenever they encounter a new word. Practice is the most
important component in making the decoding process automatic and almost
unconscious (Esher, 1997).
Through the 1980's and early 1990's, some prominent reading researchers
have argued for the intense and systematic teaching of phonics (Chall, 1983;
Adams, 1990; Stahl, 1992). According to Constance Weaver (1994), educators
agree that children learning to read written English texts must
learn that there is a
relationship between letter patterns and sound patterns in English; they also agree
that children need to develop the ability to relate letter patterns to sound patterns.
Recently, though, those staunch advocates of phonics argue that phonics is all that
children need in order to learn to read. The sources of phonics-first propaganda
back their arguments with references to respected researchers, who commonly
18
2 6
proclaim to demonstate the superiority of teaching
phonics intensively and
systematically. However,
even these researchers do not advocate the phonics-only
method as a means of teaching children
to read (Adams, 1990).
The major body of comparative research arguing
for the teaching of intense
phonics was originally that summarized by Jeanne
Chall in 1967 and updated in
1983. Chall sununarizes her findings by saying that judging
from the studies
comparing systematic phonics with intrinsic phonics (phonics
taught more
gradually, in the context of meaningful reading) it
can be concluded that
systematic phonics at the very beginning tends to produce generally
better reading
and spelling achievement than intrinsic phonics, at least through grade
three
(Chall, 1983). In this context, "achievement"
means scores on standardized tests,
that, for reading, often contain subtests of phonics knowledge. This body of
research says nothing about how children read and comprehend normal
texts
(Carbo, 1988).
The last decade has seen considerable research on phonological
awareness.
Phonological awareness is defined as the ability to hear and manipulate sound
units in the language, such as syllables, the major parts of syllables, and
phonemes. Some of this research has demonstrated that there is a strong
correlation between phonemic awareness and reading achievement, as measured
by scores on standardized tests (Adams & Bruck, 1995; Beck & Juel, 1995;
Foorman, 1995). Such correlation research has led to the argument that children
19
2 7
should be explicitly taught phonemic
awareness, not merely to help them sound
out words, but to recognize words
on sight, automatically (Stanovich, 1991, 1992).
Although phonic methods
were more prevalent before World War I, the
first sight, whole-word,
or "look and say" methods began in the 19th century.
Believing that words are recognized
as wholes and that a word's meaning is more
important that its pronunciation, William
Holmes' "McGuffey Reader"
was
developed and used until the first quarter of the 20th
century. This basal reader
approach to teaching reading first began to develop with the
introduction of
McGuffey readers in the 1830's. McGuffey readers became the
mainstay in
American education. For almost
a century they were the main reading materials
for over 80 percent of America's school children (Hart, 1950).
One of the lasting
contributions of the McGuffey was its careful graduation of material in
a series
that provided one reader for each grade of the elementary school (Gage, 1963).
From 1930 to 1960, McGuffey readers became regarded
as out-of-date,
and newer readers began to replace them. In the 1960's and 1970's,
a new style in
the basal reading series emerged. Each dimension of the reading series, from story
setting to characters to themes to subject matter, underwent dramatic change
(Steddon & Stever, 1979).
The basal reader of today represents a publisher's attempt to develop a
preplanned, sequentially organized group of materials and methods for teaching
developmental reading. Basal readers have traditionally followed the pattern of
20
2 8
McGuffey readers, which featured controlled vocabulary,
gradually increasing
difficulty, and content aimed at being interesting
to the majority of children
(Peryon, 1981).
Today the basal reader approach is the favored approach for
teaching
reading in the majority of elementary classrooms (Chall, 1983).
George and
Evelyn Spache (1977) reported that, in
a survey of 1300 teachers, 95 to 98 percent
used the basal reader approach to teach reading. The chief appeals
of the basal
reader are that it is sequential and teachers with limited backgrounds in the
teaching of reading can use it easily and successfully for beginning instruction
(Baird, 1987). Shannon (1983) suggests that teachers feel administrative
pressure
to use the basal reader and its accompanying teacher's manual as a major
component of their reading program. Parents and the community also place
pressure on teachers and the school to provide formal textbooks for their children
to learn to read.
The basal reader approach involves utilization of a carefully organized
series of books. Most series try to address all phases of the reading program,
including word recognition, vocabulary development, comprehension, oral and
silent reading, and reading for information and recreation. The teachers' manuals
give an assortment of suggestions and detailed plans for each story and include a
sequence of skills practices (Dowhover, 1989). The components of basal
programs are: student reader, teacher's manual, student workbook, practice sheets,
21
2 9
word cards, filmstrips, audio cassette tapes, and tests. The strengths include
high
structure, many approaches, scope, sequence, non-realistic
stories, and the
abundance of pre-prepared worksheets and skills-practices (Demos, 1987).
Stories found in basal readers have changed over the years as society's
image of itself has changed. Content has passed through religious,
moral,
materialistic, and, more recently, multicultural phases. Basal reader
publishers are
using more good literature and even hiring famous children's authors to
write
original stories for early reading texts. They realize that sexism, ageism,
work
mode bias, racism and stereotypic ideal family situations have no
place in
children's reading material. If readers are to understand text, basals must
be
understandable; they must be realistic (Cassidy, 1987).
Whole Language
After World War II, the heavy emphasis on skills-based reading
instruction
began to give way to a broadened concept of language learning (Kolstad
&
Bardwell, 1997). Recognizing the limitations of basic skills instruction and the
quality of reading materials that focused on a skills acquisition model
of the
reading process, school systems across the nation implemented a shift
in both
instruction and materials to what is known as a "literature-based, whole
language
approach" (Gutknecht, 1989).
The term "whole language" was coined by Dr. Kenneth Goodman of the
22
3 0
University of Arizona in the early 1980's. Whole
language developed into much
more than just a reading program. It is an educational philosophy
in its own right.
Goodman believed that learning written
language occurs naturally, in the
same
way spoken language is acquired. He thought children learn
to read primarily by
figuring out the meaning of words from analyzing
the context in which they
occur
(Esher, 1997). Although several
states and school districts had practiced whole
language, at least in part, the educational philosophy
and pedagogy first gained
widespread acceptance in California, where the
state education department
incorporated it into its English-language
arts framework in 1987 (Diegmuller,
1996). In Canada, other leaders emerged during
approximately this same time
period, among them Judith Newman and David Doake.
In New Zealand and
Australia, where whole language is known
as "natural" learning, the best-known
researchers are Don Holdaway and Brian Cambourne (Weaver,
1995). Whole
language has been described by Goodman, Bird, and Goodman
(1991) as:
nothing short of a grass-roots revolution in education. It brings together
the
scientific study of learning, of language, of teaching, and of curriculum
with the positive, people-centered, historical traditions that sensitive,
caring
teachers have always upheld. A whole language classroom is
a democratic
community of learners, and its curriculum is embedded in the culture and
social experience of the larger community (p.1).
Whole language has received a great deal of attention from the professional
community as an alternative to the traditional basal reading
program (Kolstad &
Budwell, 1997).
23
31
In part, the shift from skills-based reading instruction
to whole language
was based on investigations into the reading process that indicated that readers
create meaning from a transaction/interaction of infonnation and the
reader's
existing knowledge. From this perspective, reading is
an interchange among the
reader, the text, the environment, and the reading
process. This "receptive
language" aspect requires the reader to decode the
message of the text, encoded by
the author. The reader decodes by bringing his/her experiential
and conceptual
background of thoughts and ideas to the search for meaning in the
text (Gutknecht,
1989).
Reading instruction that can be called "whole language" must be built
on
the understanding of the nature and relationship of author, text, reader, and
language. Basic skills reading approaches
pay less attention to author and reader,
focusing almost entirely on the text and the phonemic codes of the language. A
typical description of enthusiasm for whole language
comes from Routman (1988),
who says that reading should be taught as an active
process, that students should
be actively involved with print. The elements of whole language should be used
so that students acquire an appreciation and love for reading.
In defending the trend toward whole language, researchers from the Center
for the Study of Reading reported that although current basal stories use real
children's literature, many stories seem unbelievable next to the children's own
lives and certainly dull in comparison to television. Basal stories focus on what
24
3 2
characters say and do and not
on what they are thinking and feeling, thereby
depriving children of the opportunity
to develop critical thinking strategies
(Marshall, 1983). Whole language
attempts to present students with text that is
understandable, with characters and situations within
the realm of reality
(Gutknecht, 1989).
Whole language declares that reading should
not be taught as a fragmented
series of subskills, because real language does
not exist in isolated bits and pieces.
Reading should be presented to the student
as an integrated unit composed of
listening, speaking, and writing,
as well as actual reading experience (which
should include use of the language experience approach, the
directed reading
approach, and the teacher reading to the students) (Gutknecht, 1989).
Whole
language is pragmatic in its approach, in that it is language in
use, having to do
with the reader's prior knowledge and with how language has meaning within
the
context in which it is read. Whole language involves semantics, that is the
writer's/reader's sense of meaning, that is influenced by background and culture
(Gilles, Bixby, Crowley, Crenshaw, Henrichs, Reynolds, & Pyle, 1988).
Tierney and Pearson (1985) suggested several ways to help teachers
overcome the constraints of the typical reading lesson and the "skill and drill"
approach. Teachers must fmd out the children's prior knowledge before reading
the story, encourage rereading, and give students the opportunity go evaluate their
own ideas. In addition, students must be given the opportunity to consider the
25
3 3
viewpoints of others and to recognize the influence their
own past experiences and
their own reasons for reading have on their understanding of the
text. Further,
teachers should spend time helping students develop strategies for reading,
perhaps
"mapping" the reading task prior to actual reading,
encourage students to consider
the author's message to them, develop links between what students read and what
they write, and involve students in discussion and cooperative learning, regarding
their reading assignments.
According to Constance Weaver (1995), some of the key characteristics of
whole language education are the following:
1. Acceptance of learners. All learners are accepted, regardless of their
cultural or socio-economic background or other characteristics or labels. But
"acceptance of learners" also means that whole language teachers develop
classroom environment and cuiriculum with the students, engaging them in
learning about things that interest them.
2. Flexibility within structure. Instead of having children do one worksheet
after another, whole language teachers organize the day into larger blocks of time,
so that students engage in meaningful pursuits. They study a theme or topic across
several curriculum areas. Students have many choices as to what they will do and
learn; however, the teacher guides, supports, and structures the students' learning
as needed.
3. Supportive classroom community. Teachers have the opportunity to
26
3 4
help children interact with each other, solving interpersonal
conflicts and
problems, supporting one another in learning, and
taking much responsibility for
their own behavior and learning.
4. Expectations for success
as they engage in "real" reading, writing, and
learning. Students are given the support they need to read and
write whole texts
from the very beginning. Whole language teachers have discovered that
almost all
students can learn to read and write whole texts.
5. Skills taught in context. Instead of being taught in isolation, skills
are
taught in mini-lessons and conferences, within the context of students' reading,
writing, and learning. Phonics is taught mainly through writing the sounds they
hear in the words read and reread with the teacher. Spelling and
grammar are
mainly taught while children are revising and editing their writing. In short, skills
are taught while students are engaged in real-life tasks.
6. Teacher support for learning and collaboration. Whole language
teachers collaborate with children in carrying out research projects and, in the
process, they model and explain how to do things that the children cannot yet do
alone. By collaborating on projects, children provide similar support for one
another.
7. Contextual assessment that emphasizes individuals' growth, as well as
their accomplishments. Assessment takes on a new meaning within whole
language theory. Standardized tests are not emphasized. Assessment is based
27
35
primarily upon what children are doing from day to day as they read, write,
research, and express their learning in various ways. Whole language teachers
often involve children in assessing their own work and progress and in setting
future goals for learning. Parents and peers may also be involved in assessment.
Individual growth and strengths are emphasized, within agreed-upon goals and
predetermined criteria.
Whole language represents a different philosophy about teaching, learning,
and the role of language in the classroom. It stresses that children should use
language in ways that relate to their own lives and cultures. In the whole language
classroom, the final product, the "answer," is not as important as the process
(Diegmueller, 1996). Reuling instruction must be implemented in the context of
the other language/communication processes--speaking, listening, and writing.
The content of materials must be real to the children. Finally, whole language
experts say that reading instruction has not been all wrong in the past; it has just
been starting in the wrong place. Whole language advocates proclaim that by
focusing more on the reader's knowledge and the orderly combination of related
parts of the language system, whole language instruction begins in the mind of the
reader, not with the letters on the page (Gutknecht, 1989).
A Balanced Approach to Reading Comprehension
Research in reading comprehension has received more attention in the past
28
3 6
15 years than in the previous six decades. Roger Glaser
suggests in Becoming a
Nation of Readers that the research
now available on the reading process can help
to identify what teaching practices
are most effective in the teaching of reading
comprehension (Anderson, et al., 1985). Early research
in reading comprehension
(1915 to 1970) seemed to move at
a snail's pace, perhaps reflecting the continuing
influence of the behavioral tradition that dominated psychology
during that time.
Behaviorism emphasized the study of observable behavior
or events. Since the
reading process is primarily a mental
process, much of the process is not
observable and, therefore, outside the scope of psychological testing. Research
in
reading began to assume a "product orientation," with attention given
to accuracy
in oral reading and performance on standardized tests of reading skills The results
of such testing was a undue emphasis on phonics, since the testing of the
knowledge of letter sounds, blending skills, auditory perception and discrimination
is fairly simple. The comprehension process was treated as
a "by-product," since
many assumed that comprehension automatically followed, once students has
"broken the code," and could listen to what they themselves said (Anthony,
Pearson, & Raphael, 1989).
Fortunately for those concerned with reading comprehension, the field of
psychology that had banned reading comprehension as a field of study reinstated
the study, this time in the area of cognitive psychology. Reading, considered to be
one form of problem solving, began to be studied by psychologists, linguists, and
29
anthropologists, in addition to reading educators. Several
new models of reading
instruction evolved during this period (Pearson, 1986). Gough (1972)
and his
colleagues proposed bottom-up models, emphasizing the flow of information
from
text, to visual memory, to auditory memory, to comprehension. Smith (1971) and
Goodman (1976), on the other hand, developed top-down models that emphasized
the internalization of meaning from reading. Other researchers such
as Rumelhart
(1977) and Stanovich (1980) constructed interactive models which, depending
upon the text, context, and reader, allow the flow of information to switch from
bottom-up to top-down.
The reading community defines reading comprehension
as "the process of
constructing meaning through the dynamic interaction among the reader's existing
knowledge, the information suggested by the written language, and the context of
the reading situation" (Wixson & Peters, 1984, p.4). It is representative of the
current trend away from decoding-only emphasis. Reading comprehension
involves interpreting text and constructing meaning in light of the reader's
background knowledge, his/her goals, purpose, and expectations (Anthony, et al.,
1989).
With the knowledge that a demonstration of competence in a collection of
reading skills does not equal reading, teachers understand that instructional
emphasis in reading must move from phonics-only, skill-and-drill, easy-to-teach,
easy-to-test reading instruction to application of knowledge. This means that
30
3 8
reading instruction moves from surface level word recognition skills and low level
literal comprehension skills to the interpretive and applied levels of comprehension
which enable the reader to meet the real goal of reading--understanding the
message of the author (Gutknecht, 1989).
The comprehension of what one reads is communication process, involving
the processes of speaking, listening, and writing. The purpose of this
communication is to send and receive a message from the speaker (the writer) to
the listener (the reader). Any reading instruction that stops short of this purpose, is
changed, because it is short-changing the reader. Teachers begin comprehension
instniction early, and it is built on what children already know. All children,
regardless of background, have some kind of prior knowledge. Using a top-down
or transactive/interactive approach, teachers should use meaningful, predictable
stories and ask literal and interpretive level questions, involving children in
comprehension strategies such as stating main idea, grouping and classifying
information, sequencing, and predicting outcomes (Gutknecht, 1989).
Learning to read is a critical basic skill, yet its accomplishment among
American children is by no means automatic or universal (Kolstad & Bardwell,
1997). The most striking fmding from the 1994 National Assessment of
Educational Progress (NAEP) was that the average reading proficiency of twelfth-
grade students declined significantly from 1992 to 1994.
No consensus exists on just how reading is learned. Most students seem to
31
3 9
master phonics and other basal reader skills perfectly in the lower grades. But a
common occurrence in high school classrooms is to have students who can
"decode" a reading passage flawlessly, have perfect pronunciation, but do not
comprehend what they read. Because this situation is so very commonwith
students who master their phonics perfectly, but cannot adequately comprehend
what they so easily pronounceeducators look to engage students in what they are
learning so that reading becomes more meaningful (Cirone, 1997).
As previously stated, the term "whole language" is often used to cover a
process designed to immerse students in literature and related writing activities.
Whole language is effective with most children, but some respond better to other
methods. Teachers cannot be limited to only one method of teaching reading
(Kolstad & Bardwell, 1997). As Church (1994) stated, "I have faced the
uncomfortable reality that there really are no certainties in teaching - no right way.
Some students learn best from part to whole, others learn better using the holistic
method (whole to part)" (p.362).
Using whole language to the exclusion of phonics and basic reading skills
was never the intention of those who developed the
theory of whole language. It
used literature that was of interest to young people in order to engage them in the
process more rapidly, and to enrich reading programs
that frequently offered
unending "drill-and-kill" skills that were far removed from the purpose of getting
meaning from what was read. But it never meant to replace phonics, and those
32
4 0
few who used it to extreme were as misguided as those who would
not supplement
the basal readers with literature. Conversely, reading and understanding
great
literature are also of little use if one cannot express oneself with good
pronunciation and clear spelling. Students must be taught both phonics and
comprehension skills (Cirone, 1997). Speigel (1992) stated that bridges
can and
must be built between whole language and more traditional literacy instruction to
enable teachers to blend the best of both in order to help
every child reach his or
her full literacy potential. She says that teachers will draw what works from
a
variety of approaches.
In California, where whole language first gained widespread acceptance
into language arts instruction, the state board of education has been force to
rethink its approach to reading instruction. Placing much of the blame for dismal
standardized test scores on the almost exclusive teaching of whole language, the
state board last year rejected all materials thatit felt lacked sufficient emphasis on
basic reading skills. Yvonne W. Larson, the chairperson of the board, said in an
interview, "What we are looking for is a balanced approach...We want voracious
readers, but our feeling is they've got to have the basic skills first." California
education leaders pledged that they would work toward promoting a balanced
approach by having teachers blend basic skills instruction with exposure to rich
literature and writing (Manzo, 1997).
Astute teachers who favor a balanced approach to whole language are
33
41
aware that, given the same text, readers do not necessarily construct the same
meaning. This individuality is recognized, respected, and utilized in the literacy
program. With this understanding, teachers are careful not to label readers as low,
average, high. They realize that all students are more proficient when reading
something of interest than when they are involved in artificial reading material that
reaps no benefits or joy. Students realize this fact, also. The teacher's task then
becomes, not to stamp out readers' miscues, but rather to help students to establish
and control their reading strategies. When readers gain control of the
process,
their miscues, their comprehension, and their critical thinking get better (Gilles, et
al., 1988).
Conclusion
While it is interesting to debate the pros and cons of various approaches to
the teaching of reading, it is meaningless and counterproductive to do so. Any
system that eliminates literature and comprehension skills altogether is as useless
as one that eliminates phonics and basic skills. Educators should make this clear
whenever possible to the public. Different students have different reading needs,
educators must have the flexibility to diagnose needs in a given case, then provide
the necessary teaching tools (Cirone, 1997).
Society places a high priority on literacy. Reading instruction in both
elementary and secondary schools has undergone many changes throughout the
34
4 2
years. Many of these changes have resulted in gains in reading achievement.
Despite these gains, schools
are constantly searching for innovative approaches
and programs that will reach all students. This is
necessary since some students
are still functionally illiterate at the time of high school graduation. The goal of all
school systems and teachers is for the improvement of reading
in all children.
Since there is no single method that
seems appropriate for all children, the
recommendation of most current research is that
a combination of these
approaches be used in the teaching of reading. Balance and individualization
have
always been (and still are) the keys to teaching children
to read (VanProoyen &
Clouse, 1994).
Chapter 3
METHOD
Sub'ects
North Marion High School, from which the sample
was taken, is located in
rural, north-central West Virginia, with
a largely middle and lower socio-economic
population. The high school consists of 1056 students and 64 classroom teachers.
The sample consisted of five intact high school remedial reading classes
called English/Reading, taught by one teacher with 18
years experience, eight of
which have been in the area of remedial reading. Students in grades nine through
twelve, whose reading comprehension test scores are considerably below their
grade level, are identified and randomly assigned to the English/Reading classes
through computer scheduling.
Because of this random class selection and in order not to disrupt
institutional routine, the 27 students in three of the five English/Reading classes
were chosen to be the control group, and the 27 students in the other two classes
were chosen to be the experimental group. The control group of 18 boys and 9
girls received traditional, skills-based reading instruction, using workbooks and
work packets. The experimental group of 20 boys and 7 girls received whole
language instruction, using the novel.
36
4 4
Design
The research design used
was quasi-experimental because of the use of
randomly assigned groups of subjects, rather than randomly
assigned individual
subjects. Since the quasi-experimental design
was used, a pretest was given before
the study began, to determine the homogeneity of the
intact control group and the
intact experimental group with respect to reading comprehension
(the dependent
variable). Similarly, at the completion of the experiment,
a reading comprehension
post-test was administered to assess the similarities/differences between the
two
intact groups, each having been exposed to different independent
variables
(methods of teaching).
The Reading Comprehension Test portion of Advanced 1 Form L of the
Metropolitan Achievement Test was administered
as a pretest to analyze and
compare the control group and the experimental group. An independent t-test was
run at the .05 level to determine if the two groups were homogeneous in the area
of reading comprehension.
The post-test data collected from the results of the Reading Comprehension
Test of Advanced 1 Form M (Mat6) was employed to determine how significant
a
difference in test scores occurred in the two groups at the end of the study. An
independent t-test was run at the .05 level on the results to determine if significant
differences occurred.
37
4 5
Experimental Design
Assignment N Pretest
Treatment
Post-Test
Control
27 MAT6 Form L Skills-based
MAT6 Form M
Experimental 27 MAT6 Form L Whole
Language MAT6 Form M
Two separate dependent t-tests,
run at the .05 level, were also used to
analyze the pretest and post-test data from
both the control
group and the
experimental group to determine if the students'
reading comprehension
was
significantly better at the end of the study.
Procedure
At the beginning of the 1997-1998 school
year, the 54 students comprising
the control group and the experimental
group had been randomly placed in five
English/Reading classrooms by computer scheduling. The ten-week
experiment
began the first part of February, 1998, and
was completed mid-April, 1998.
The Reading Comprehension Test of the Metropolitan
Achievement Test,
Advanced 1 Form L, was administered to all 54 students prior
to the inception of
the experiment. Twenty-seven students were included in the control
group which
received traditional, skills-based instruction in remedial reading comprehension.
The other 27 students were included in the experimental
group which received
remedial reading instruction using high-interest, low-level novels.
The daily
38
4 6
instniction in the skills-based, control group included the
use of various sets of
workbooks and work packets that teach reading comprehension
as a series of
isolated sldlls: Specific Skills Series (Following Directions, Using the Context,
Locating the Answer, Getting the Facts, Getting the Main Idea, Drawing
Conclusions, Detecting Sequence, Identifying Inferences), Reading for Concepts,
New Practice Readers, and Reading and Critical Thinking in the Content Areas.
During the ten-week period, the instructor, rotated the skills workbooks, focusing
the students on one comprehension skill at a time and providing multiple practices
in each skill.
During the same period, the experimental group received instruction in the
guided reading of three novels. The first novel, The Outsiders,
was read orally and
by the entire group. Activities that accompanied the chapter readings
were
vocabulary in context, questions for discussion and written responses in finding
facts and critical evaluation, story mapping, character analysis, prediction, analysis
of the elements of the novel, comparing/contrasting the novel with the video, and a
doze procedure final test. Two additional Tale Spinner novels were chosen by
each individual student as independent study projects. Students were asked to
mimic several of the activities from The Outsiders unit, summarizing chapters,
developing their own vocabulary list, finding main idea, predicting and sequencing
events, making inferences, and drawing conclusions.
The Reading Comprehemsion Test of Advanced 1 Form M (MAT6) was
39
4 7
given as a post-test to both groups of students in order to determine if any
significant difference in reading comprehension occurred in the two groups at the
end of the ten-week study.
Instrumentation
The Reading Comprehension Test portion of the Metropolitan Achievement
Test, Sixth Edition, (MAT6) was the instrument used in this research. The test
contains 60 items that measure students' comprehension of reading passages. The
reading levels of the 10 passages begin at grade 5 and increase in difficulty to
grade 11+. Though the reading selections in Form L and Form M are different,
both forms are of equal length and difficulty. The 60 item multiple-choice test
assesses students' ability to recognize detail and sequence; infer meaning, cause
and effect, main idea, and character analysis; and draw conclusions, determine
author's purpose, and distinguish fact from opinion.
Objective criteria for the Advanced 1 Level of the MAT6 are in the form of
a raw score, percentile rank, stanine, grade equivalent, and scaled score. (Form M
of the MAT6 follows Form L.) For the purpose of this study, students' raw scores
were used.
40
4 8
Chapter 4
ANALYSIS OF DATA
This chapter presents the results of the study. The data collected that is
appropriate to the research question and the implications of that data
are discussed.
Statement of the Problem
The purpose of this research was to analyze and compare two methods of
teaching reading comprehension in a remedial reading classroom. One approach
implemented a traditional, skills-based approach to remedial reading. The
alternative approach was an integrated, whole-language approach to remedial
reading using the novel. Two groups of high school remedial reading students
were used in the study. The Metropolitan Achievement Test was the instrument
used to evaluate the reading comprehension of the two groups, both before and
after the period of the study, to determine if any significant difference existed in
student performance
Subjects
The control group in the study consisted of 27 students enrolled in English/
Reading classes at North Marion High School who received traditional, skills-
based reading instruction. Sample data for the control group is located in
41
4 9
Appendix A. The experimental
group included 27 students in English/Reading at
the same location who received whole language
instruction. Sample data for the
experimental group is located in Appendix B.
The gender make-up of the samples is found in Table
1. The control
sample consisted of 18 males and 9 females, while 20 males
and 7 females made
up the experimental sample. This data is graphed in Appendix C.
TABLE 1
Gender of Students in Samples
Group
Number
Males
Females
Control
27
18
9
.
,
Experimental 27
20
7
_
The grade level data of the students in the samples are organized in Table 2.
The control group was made up of 7 seniors, 7 juniors, 4 sophomores, and 9
freshmen. The experimental group consisted of 7 seniors, no juniors, 9
sophomores, and 11 freshmen. The graphs in Appendix D show this data.
42
5 0
TABLE 2
Grade Levels of Students in Samples
Group
Number
Seniors Juniors
Sophomores
Freshmen
Control
27 7
7 4 9
Experimental
27
7
0 9
11
The ages of the students in the sample ranged from nineteen to fifteen.
Table 3 exhibits the age data. The control group consisted of three 19-year-olds,
seven 18-year-olds, four 17-year-olds, three 16-year-olds, and ten 15-year-olds.
Zero 19-year-olds, seven 18-year-olds, five 17-year-olds, nine 16-year-olds, and
six 15-year-olds made up the experimental group. This data is graphed in
Appendix E.
TABLE 3
Ages of Students in Samples
Group
Number
19
18 17
16
15
Control
27
3 7
4
3 10
Experimental
27
0
7
5
9
6
43
51
Homogeneity
The Reading Comprehension Test of Advanced 1 Form L of the
Metropolitan Achievement Test was administered to both groups of students as a
pretest to analyze and compare the reading comprehension level of students in the
study. Pretest data is located in Appendix F of this study. Using the raw data
given in Appendix F, an independent t-test was run at the .05 level to determine if
the two groups were homogeneous in the area of reading comprehension. Based
on the results of this t-test, as shown in Table 4, the
assumption was made that the
groups were similar; therefore, the study continued.
TABLE 4
Pretest Scores
Reading Comprehension
.
Group
Number
Mean
T-score
Control
27
34.85
Experimental
27
33.67
T-test
-0.4339
Since the t-score was -0.4439, and the critical t-value was 2.0066, the
44
5 2
sample provided enough evidence to accept the null hypothesis,
indicating no
significant difference at the .05 level, in the reading comprehension
pretest scores
of the control group and the experimental
group.
Further analysis of the raw data on the pretest from Appendix F indicated
that the highest number scored by any individual student in the control
group in
reading comprehension was 48 of a possible 60. The lowest
score was 17, and the
average score 34.85. In the experimental group the high score was 51, the low
score was 12, and the average score was 33.67.
The Hypothesis
Students in a high school remedial reading classroom who receive whole
language instruction using the novel and students who receive
a traditional, skills-
based direction will score equally on a standardized reading comprehension test.
Findings and Interpretations
The ten-week experiment began the first part of February, 1998, and was
completed by mid-April, 1998. During this period, the control group received
traditional, skills-based reading instruction, while the experimental group was
instructed using the whole language method. The Reading Comprehension Test of
the Metropolitan Achievement Test, Advanced 1 Form M, was administered as a
post-test to both the control group and the experimental group in mid-April, 1998.
45
5 3
The groups consisted of the
same students used in the pretest phase of the study.
Post-test data is located in Appendix G of this
study. Using the raw data given in
Appendix G, an independent t-test
was run at the .05 level to determine if the null
hypothesis would be accepted
or rejected in the area of reading comprehension.
The results of the t-test are shown in Table 5.
TABLE 5
Post-test Scores
Reading Comprehension
Group
Number
Mean
T-score
Control
27
39.481
Experimental
27
39.037
T-test
-0.1601
With the t-score of -0.1601 and the critical t-value of 2.0066, the sample
provided enough evidence to accept the null hypothesis, indicating
no significant
difference at the .05 level, in the reading comprehension post-test
scores of the
control group and the experimental group.
Further analysis of the raw data on the post-test from Appendix G indicated
that the highest number scored by an individual student in the control group in
46
5 9
reading comprehension was 57 of a possible 60, while the lowest
score was 26,
with an average score of 39.481. In the experimental
group the high score was
also 57, the low score was 12
,
and the average score was 39.037.
In addition, two separate dependent t-tests, run at the .05 level,
were
administered to analyze the pretest and post-test data within each
group to
determine if students' reading comprehension
was significantly improved at the
end of the study. The results of this test are shown in Table 6.
TABLE 6
Comparison of Pretest and Post-test Scores
Reading Comprehension
Group
Number
Mean Difference
T-score
Control
27
-4.630
-3.6550
Experimental
27
-5.370
-4.6853
With a critical t-value of 2.0555 and t-scores of -3.6550 and -4.6853, the
sample provided enough evidence to reject the null hypothesis, indicating a
significant improvement in reading comprehension scores within both groups from
pretest to post-test.
47
5 5
Chapter 5
SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS
Summary
This study was conducted at North Marion High School during the 1997-
1998 school year. The purpose of the research was to analyze and compare
reading comprehension test results of two groups of high school remedial reading
students. The experimental group of students received reading instruction
implementing an integrated, whole-language approach to remedial reading using
the novel, while the control group was exposed to a more traditional, skills-based
approach to remedial reading. The 27 students in each group were assigned
numbers to keep scores confidential. The Metropolitan Achievement Test was the
measuring device used to collect pretest and post-test data for purposes of analysis
and comparison.
Sample data concerning the gender, age, and grade level of the students in
both groups were collected, charted, and graphed. A t-test was run comparing the
pretest scores of the control group to those of the experimental group to determine
if the two groups were homogeneous. At the .05 level, no significant difference
was found. This indicated that the two groups were homogeneous in the area of
reading comprehension. Following the 10-week experiment in which each group
received different methods of instruction, a t-test was run on the post-test scores of
48
5 6
the two groups to determine if any significant difference could be found in the
students reading comprehension. At the .05 level, no significant difference in
reading comprehension was found in the two groups, taught by two different
methods.
Conclusions
The pros and cons of various methods of teaching reading have been
debated frequently in various education journals. In the past, most school systems
advocated the exclusive use of basal readers and the intense use of phonics. After
World War II, many school systems adopted a new and innovative approach to
reading called whole language.
The results of this study would seem to indicate that the method of
instruction had no significant effect on the reading comprehension test scores of
students in a high school remedial reading classroom. In this study students taught
using whole language and students taught using skills-based instruction scored
equally as well on the Metropolitan Achievement Test
Since no significant difference was shown in the two methods of reading
instruction in this study, it would seem logical to conclude that a balanced
approach to teaching reading comprehension would be most beneficial to high
school remedial reading students.
49
57
Recommendations
For the purpose of future studies in the methods
of teaching high school
remedial reading, the following recommendations
are made:
1. The sample in this study comprised
a small group of students from one high
school. Future studies could include
more students and be conducted in more than
one high school.
2. The time period of this study
was 10 weeks. A similar study encompassing a
longer period of time between pretest and
post-test might be done to examine
significant differences.
3. Using the same data collected in this study,
an analysis could be done
comparing the test results of males to females. Test results of students of the
same
grade level or of the same age could be compared and analyzed.
4. A similar study could be done under the direction and instruction of another
or
several other teachers and the results compared.
5. A similar study, using less difficult versions of the Metropolitan Achievement
Test, could be done at the elementary or middle school level, to ascertain if there
is a significant difference in methods of reading instruction at different levels of
education.
6. A different testing instrument could be used for evaluation purposes in a similar
study.
7. Since the students in this sample are also required to take the Stanford
50
58
Achievement Test each year, reading comprehension scores from that test could be
compared to results found in this study.
8. Since the Metropolitan Achievement Test provides an item analysis, a study
could be conducted, analyziiig student weaknesses in comprehension areas such as
detail and sequence, inference, cause and effect, main idea, character analysis,
drawing conclusions, author's purpose, and fact and opinion.
9. After being exposed to both skills-based reading instruction and whole
language, the students involved in the study could be surveyed to determine which
method they preferred and which method they found to be most beneficial to them.
5 1
5 9
REFERENCES
Aaron, I. E. (1961). Teaching word recognition skills in Georgia Schools.
Atlanta, GA: State Department of Education.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Thinking and learning about print.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Adams, M. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Resolving the "Great Debate." American
Educator. 19 (2). 7,10-20.
Anderson, R., Hiebert, E., Scott, J., & Wilkinson, I. (1985). Becoming
a nation of
readers: The report of the Commission
on Reading (ME Contract No. 400-
83-0057). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Anthony, H. M., Pearson, P.D., & Raphael, T. E. (1989). geading
comprehension research: A selected review. (Technical Report No. 448).
Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 302 827)
Baird, S. R. (1987). The first r: Fundamentals of initial reading instruction.
Development in classroom instruction. Washington, DC: National
Education Association.
Beck, I. L., & Juel, C. (1995). The role of decoding in learning to read.
American Educator. 19 (2), 8, 21-25, 39-42.
Bloomfield, L. (1942). Linguistics and reading. Elementary English Review,
19(April/May), 125-130, 183-186.
Calfee, R. C., & Drum, P. A. (1979). Teaching reading in compensatory classes.
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Carbo, M. (1988). Debunking the great phonics myth. Phi Delta Kappan. 70,
226-240.
Carbone, T. H., & Evans, M. A. (1985). Life beyond the basal. Illinois Schools
JournaL 67, 49-55.
Cassidy, K. S. (1987, September). Basals are better. Learning87, 65-66.
52
6 0
Chall, J. (1983). Learning to read: The great debate. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Chall, J. S., Popp, H. M., & Hirshberg, J. (1993). Reading.
Encyclopedia
Americana (Deluxe Library Edition).
Danbury, CN: Grolier Incorporated.
Church, S. (1994). Is whole language really
warm and fuzzy? The Reading
Teacher. 47., 362-369.
Cirone, B. (1997). Balancing phonics and whole language. Thrust for
Educational Leadership. 26, 20-21.
Commission on Excellence. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative for
educational reform. (Contract No. 065-000-00177-2). Washington, DC:
Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office.
Diegmueller, K. (1996, March). A war of words: Whole language under siege.
Education Week on the Web. [On-line]. Available:
http://www.edweek.org/ew/vo1-15126eng.h15
Dechant, E. (1982). Improving the teaching_of reading (3rd ed.). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
Demos, E. (1987). School based reading programs. Reading Horizons. 27, 189-
196.
Dowhover, S. L. (1989). A principal's guide to reading terminology. Principal.
January. 36-38.
Esher, M. (1997, March). What is whole language? Arizona Parents for
Traditional Education. [On-line]. Available:
http://www.theriver.com/Publickucson_parents_eduforum/wholelanguag
e.html
Fay, L. (1956). Reading in the high school: What research says to the teacher.
Washington, DC: NEA Department of Classroom Teachers and American
Educational Research Association.
Flesch, R. (1981). Why Johnny still can't read. New York: Harper and Row.
Flexner, S. B. (Ed.). (1987). Random House dictionary of the English language
(2nd edition unabridged). New York: Random House.
53
61
Foorman, B. R. (1995). Research
on "The great debate": Code oriented
versus
whole language approaches to reading instruction.
School Psychology
Review. 24., 376-392.
Gage, N.L. (1963). Handbook of research
on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally
& Company.
Gilles, C., Bixby, M., Crowley, P., Crenshaw, S.,
Henrichs, M., Reynolds, F., &
Pyle, D. (1988). Whole language strategies for
secondary students.
Katonah, NY: Richard C. Owen Publishers, Inc.
Goodman, K.S. (1976). Behind the
eye: What happens in reading. In H. Singer
& R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and
processes of reading.
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Goodman, K. S., Bird, L. B., & Goodman, Y. M. (1991). What
is whole
language? The whole language catalog. [On-line]. Available:
http://www.u.arizona. eduk-vweldon/ 4.html
Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In J. F. Kavanaugh
& I. G.
Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and
eye. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gutknecht, B. (1989). From basic skills to whole language: Can
we get there from
here? Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. (ERIC
Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 319 017)
Hart, J. D. (1950). The popular book: A history of America's literary
taste. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Kolstad, R. & Baldwell, J. (1997). Phonics vs. whole language in the teaching of
reading. Reading Improvement. 34., 154-160.
Leu, D., & Kinzer, C. (1987). Effective reading instruction in the elementary
grades. Columbus, OH: Merrill Publishing Company.
Manzo, K. K. (1997). Calif text adoption puts emphasis on phonics. Education
Week. 16, 15.
Marshall, K. (1983, April/May). The reading problem: Some sensible solutions.
Leas,, 37-39.
54
6 2
National Assessment of Educational Progress
(NAEP). (1994). The reading
report card: Progess toward excellence in
our schools. Champaign, IL:
University of Illinois.
Pearson, P. D. (1986). Twenty
years of research in reading comprehension. In T.
E. Raphael (Ed.), The contexts of school-based
literacy. New York:
Random House.
Peryon, C. D. (1981). Proceedings of the annual
symposium on reading
education. (Report No. CS00-7267) (ERIC Document
Reproduction
Service No. ED 236 537)
Psacharopoulos, G. (1981). Return to education: An
updated international
comparison. Comparative Education Journal. 17. 321-341.
Routman, R. (1988). Transitions: From literacy
to literacy. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann Educational Books, Inc.
Rumelhart, D. (1977). Toward an interactive model of reading.
In H. Singer &
R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and
processes of reading.
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Rumelhart, D. (1986). In H. Singer & R. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical
models and
processes of reading (3rd ed.) (722-750).
Shannon, P. (1983). The use of commercial reading materials in American
elementary schools. Reading Research Ouarterly, 19. 68-85.
Shannon, P. (1984). Mastery learning in reading and the control of teachers and
students. Language Arts. 61, 484-493.
Shulinan, L. (1983). Autonomy and obligation: The remote control of teaching.
In L. Shuman & G. Sykes (Eds.), Handbook of Teaching and Policy (484-
504). New York: Longman.
Smith, F (1971). Understanding reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Smith, R., & Johnson, D. (1980). Teaching children to read. Boston: Addison
Wesley Publishing
55
6 3
Spache, G. D., & Spache, E. B. (1977). Reading in the elementary
school (4th
ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Spiegel, D. L. (1992). Blending whole language and systematic
direct instruction.
The Reading Teacher, 46. 38-44.
Stahl, S. A. (1992). Saying the "p" word: Nine guidelines for
exemplary phonics
instruction. The Reading Teacher. 45, 618-625.
Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of
individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading
Research Quarterly. 16, 32-71.
Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Word recognition: Changing perspectives. In R.
Barr,
M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading
research (Vol. 2, pp. 307-342). New York: Longman.
Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and
consequences of
individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C.
Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), ilegAngAguisition (pp. 307-342). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Stauffer, R. G. (1980). The language experience approach to the teaching of
reading (2nd ed.). New York: Harper and Row.
Steddon, S. S., & Stever, L. 0. (1979). From McGuffey to the eighties:
American basic reading programs. Teacher. 96, 58-66.
Tierney, R. & Pearson, P. D. (1985, April/May). New priorities for teaching
reading. Learning, 14-17.
VanProoyen, N. & Clouse, R. W. (1994). Three approaches to teaching reading:
Basal. language experience, and computer-assisted instruction.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 366 910)
Weaver, C. (1995). On the nature of whole language education. Creating support
for effective literacy education. [On-line]. Available: http://www.edu.
yorku. ca/%7Ew1u108894f6.htm
56
6 4
Weaver, C. (1996). Reconsidering
a balanced approach to reading. Urbana, IL:
National Council of Teachers of English.
Winograd, P., & Greenlee, M. (1986). Children need
a balanced reading
program. Educational Leadership. 43, 16-21.
Wixson, K. K. & Peters, C. W. (1984). Reading redefined:
A Michigan Reading
Association position paper. The Michigan Reading Journal,
17, 4-7.
Wolf, M. (1993). Reading Education. New Grolier
Multimedia Encyclopedia.
(Release 6). Danbury, CN: Grolier Electronic Publisher,
Inc.
Woodward, S. (1986). Over-programmed materials: Taking the
teacher out of
teaching. American Educator. Spring. 26-31.
57
Appendix A
Control Group Data
SUBJECT
GENDER
GRADE
AGE
1
12
19
2
9
15
3
12
18
4
11
17
5
9
17
6
9
15
7
12
18
8
12
19
9
9
15
10
11
18
11
9
15
12
10
16
13
10
15
14
12
19
15
11
17
16
9
15
17
9
15
18
11
18
19
12
18
20
10
16
21
11 16
22
9
15
23
10
15
24
9
15
25
12 18
26
11 18
27
11 17
58
66
Appendix B
Experimental Group Data
SUBJECT
GENDER
GRADE
AGE
I
M
10
16
2
M
9
16
3
M
12
18
4
F
9
16
5
M
10
16
6
M
9
16
7
M
10
17
8
M
9
15
9
F
12
18
10
M
10
17
I 1
F
12
18
12
M
12
17
13
M
9
15
14
M
9
16
15
M
10
18
16
M
10
17
17
M
9
15
18
F
9
15
19
M
9
16
20
F 9
15
21
M
9
15
22
M
10
16
23
F
12
18
24
F
12
18
25
M
10
16
26
M
10
17
27
M
12
18
59
6 7
Appendix C
Control Group
Gender
Giris (33.33%)
Boys (66.67%)
Experimental Group
Gender
Giris (25.93%)-'
60
6 8
Boys (74.07%)
Appendix D
Control Group
Grade
Twelfth (25.93%
Eleventh (25.93%
inth (33.33%)
Tenth (14.81%)
Experimental Group
Grade
Eleventh (0.00%
61
6 9
Appendix E
Control Group
Age
Nineteen (11.11%)
Eighteen (25.93%)
Fifteen (37.04%)
Seventeen (14.81%)
Sixteen (11.11%)
Experimental Group
Age
Eighteen (25.93%)
Seventeen (18.52%)
Nineteen (0.00%)
Fifteen (22.22%)
62
7 0
Sixteen (33.33%)
Appendix F
Reading Comprehension
Pre-Test Scores*
SUBJECT
CONTROL GROUP EXPERIMENTAL GROUP
1 36
21
2 38
23
3 27
24
4
17 35
5 24 12
6
24
13
7 43 38
8
40 39
9
30
51
10
33
28
11
47 26
12
27
20
13
47
28
14
33
42
15
46 50
16
34
ao
17
25
42
18 31
48
19 36
24
20
32
32
21
39
41
22
32
46
23 41
37
24
32
46
25
46
24
26
33
28
27
as
51
*possible score of 60
63
71
Appendix G
Reading Comprehension
Post-Test Scores*
SUBJECT
CONTROL GROUP
EXPERIMENTAL GROUP
1
44
31
2
26
32
3
35
30
4
20
42
5
26
21
6
28
12
7
39
44
8
43
37
9
45
57
10
38
42
11
57
48
12
32
20
13
52
40
14
41
51
15
47
51
16
34
47
17
34
47
18
46
44
19
43
27
20
32
35
21
43
44
22
35
55
23
53
41
24
47
39
25
36
26
26
41
38
27
49
53
* possible score of 60
64
7
C-SO
U.S. Department of Education
Office ofEducational Research and Improvement (OERI)
National Library of Education (NLE)
Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC)
REPRODUCTION RELEASE
(Specific Document)
I. DOCUMENT IDENTIFICATION:
ERIC
Title: Ski
li5- based or NOole LanrusePeadint3 Instruct-Con
Corrparatrile,
51-tta5 ah the Trn pro Yen, enro-t
Y-fieRe4dirp0 Corn preAerts tern 6+ MjASaitoot Stzaciet4s
Author(s): Mar9are1t S
Wi tson
Corporate Source: -
II. REPRODUCTION RELEASE:
Publication Date:
In order to disseminate as widely as possible timely and significant materials of interest to the educational community, documents announced in the
monthly abstract journal of the ERIC system, Resoumes in Education (RIE), are usually made available to users in microfiche, reproduced paper copy,
and electronic media, and sold through the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDRS). Credit is given to the source of each document, and, if
reproduction release is granted, one of the following notices is affixed to the document.
If permission is granted to reproduce and disseminate the identified document, please CHECK ONE of the following three options and sign at the bottom
of the page.
The sample sticker shown below will be
affixed to all Level 1 documents
1
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS
BEEN GRANTED BY
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
Level 1
Check here for Level 1 release, permitting reproduction
and dissemination in microfiche or other ERIC archival
media (e.g., electronic) and paper copy.
Sign
here,-)
please
The sample sticker shown below will be
The sample sticker shown below will be
affixed to all Level 2A documents
affixed to all Level 28 documents
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL IN
MICROFICHE, AND IN ELECTRONIC MEDIA
FOR ERIC COLLECTION SUBSCRIBERS ONLY,
HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
\e
So's)
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
2A
Level 2A
Check here for Level 2A release, permitting reproduction
and dissemination in microfiche and in electronic media
for ERIC archival collection subscribers only
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL IN
MICROFICHE ONLY HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
2B
Level 2B
Check here for Level 28 release, permitting
reproduction and dissemination in microfiche only
'Documents will be processed as indicated provided reproductiOn quality permits.
If permission to reproduce is granted, but no box is checked, documents will be processed at Level 1.
l hereby grant to the Educational Resources Informafion Center (ER1C) nonexclusive pennission to reproduce and disseminate this document
as indicated above. Reproduction from the ERIC microfiche or electronic media by persons other than ERIC employees
and its system
contractors requires permission from the copyright holder. Exception is made for non-profit reproduction by libraries and other service agencies
to satisfy information needs of educators in response to discmte inquiries.
Signature:
Organization/Mdre
P.0 .
Box IS
V 24510
4.911444-74.)
Printed Name/Position/Titte:
M4a-9are-t S. 1/Vi lson
ne:
FAX
n4-14(19-13113
E-Maii Address:,
troltrVih 10 access.
Date: 9..16...q8
vnotintarn. net
(over)
III. DOCUMENT AVAILABILITY INFORMATION
(FROM NON-ERIC SOURCE):
If permission to reproduce is not granted to ERIC, or, if
you wish ERIC to cite the availability of the document from another
source, please
provide the following information regarding the availability of the
document. (ERIC will not announce a document unless it is
publicly
available, and a dependable source can be specified. Contributors
should also be aware that ERIC selection criteria
are significantly more
stringent for documents that cannot be made available through
EDRS.)
Publisher/Distributor:
Address:
Price:
IV. REFERRAL OF ERIC TO
COPYRIGHT/REPRODUCTION RIGHTS HOLDER:
If the right to grant this reproduction release is
held by someone other than the addressee, please provide
the appropriate name and
address:
Name:
Address:
V. WHERE TO SEND THIS
FORM:
Send this form to the following ERIC Clearinghouse:
However, if solicited by the ERIC Facility,
or if making an unsolicited contribution to ERIC, return this form (and
the document being
contributed) to:
ERIC Processing and Reference Facility
1100 West Street, 2" Floor
Laurel, Maryland 20707-3598
Telephone: 301-497-4080
Toll Free: 800-799-3742
FAX: 301-953-0263
WWW: http://ericfac.piccard.csc.com
EFF-088 (Rev. 9/97)
PREVIOUS VERSIONS OF THIS F6RM
ARE OBSOLETE.