What Do Students Remember About Introductory Psychology,
Years Later?
Bridgette Martin Hard
and Joshua M. Lovett
Duke University
Shannon T. Brady
Wake Forest University
A course in introductory psychology (Intro Psych) has potential to cultivate valuable
knowledge and skills, especially for first-year students. The present research is a first
step at understanding this potential by surveying college seniors who took Intro Psych
during their first year. We aimed to discover students’ perspectives on what they
learned, a sense of how much they actually retained, and whether students’ course
experiences predicted their academic behaviors as seniors. When asked what they
learned from the course that helped them in other courses and in life, both majors and
nonmajors described how Intro Psych shaped their study skills, knowledge, and
perspectives on the social world. In a retest of items from their first midterm, students
showed high performance (70% correct) compared to previous studies, with psychol-
ogy students (majors and minors) showing better performance than nonpsychology
students (81% vs. 69%), d .74. Finally, we found that students’ performance on a
multiphase term project in Intro Psych predicted their self-reported project management
skills as seniors (r .32, p .001). Students’ enjoyment of the course, measured by
their evaluations of the instructors, predicted self-reported project management skills
(r(148) .294, p .001) and also later study skills (r(148) .266, p .001).
Together, these findings support the importance of Intro Psych and highlight the need
to further clarify and expand our knowledge of its long-term impact.
Keywords: Intro Psych, knowledge retention, long-term outcomes
Introductory psychology (hereafter Intro
Psych) is the gateway course to the discipline of
psychology, introducing millions of college stu-
dents each year to a field that tackles fundamen-
tal questions about human behavior and expe-
rience using scientific methods. Due to both its
reach and its content, Intro Psych is poised to
make a lasting impact on students’ education,
and potentially their lives. But what do students
learn from Intro Psych? What, if anything, lin-
gers after the final exam: terms and definitions,
skills, and/or new perspectives on the world?
The American Psychological Association
(2014) has developed guidelines for Intro Psych
that suggest what students should learn (Gurung et
al., 2016). These guidelines recommend that Intro
Psych courses be designed on a foundation of
scientific methods, cover content representing five
major psychological domains (biological, cogni-
tive, developmental, social and personality, and
mental and physical health), and incorporate spe-
cific cross-cutting themes that include, among oth-
ers, the application of psychology to everyday life
and real-world situations. Accordingly, students
should walk away from Intro Psych with en-
hanced scientific literacy, foundational knowledge
representing the breadth of psychology, and the
ability to apply this knowledge to their lives. Oth-
ers have recommended that the course should also
develop essential skills, such as interpersonal and
This article was published Online First January 10, 2019.
Bridgette Martin Hard and Joshua M. Lovett, Department
of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University; Shan-
non T. Brady, Department of Psychology, Wake Forest
University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Bridgette Martin Hard, Department of Psychol-
ogy and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC
27708. E-mail: [email protected]
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 5, No. 1, 61–74
2332-2101/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/stl0000136
61
critical thinking skills (Landrum, 2017; Strohmetz
et al., 2015).
Scholars clearly have high expectations for
the big ideas and skills that students should
develop in Intro Psych. In the present study, we
sought to understand the long-term influence of
Intro Psych by surveying senior undergraduates
who completed an Intro Psych course in their
first year of college. In the sections that follow,
we first describe why Intro Psych has the po-
tential to be such an influential course, particu-
larly for first-year students. Next, we describe
what we know, and do not, about what students
gain from the course. Finally, we present our
empirical approach, which examined what stu-
dents think they learned from Intro Psych, a
sense of what they actually retained, and
whether their experiences in the course as first-
years predicted their academic behaviors as se-
niors.
The Significance of Intro Psych
Intro Psych is the second most popular un-
dergraduate course in the United States (Lan-
drum & Gurung, 2013). Its popularity extends
to high schools: Nearly one in three high school
students take a psychology course by the time
they graduate (Nord et al., 2011). The reach of
the course is only expected to increase, given
the projected growth in employment for psy-
chology-related professions (nearly 20% by
2024; Clay, 2017) and a rising emphasis on
psychology for students going into medicine
(Mitchell, Lewis, Satterfield, & Hong, 2016).
Not only is Intro Psych popular, but it has the
potential to powerfully influence the millions of
students who take it. The course imparts content
that is relevant both to students’ developing
knowledge about themselves and others and to a
wide range of other disciplines. As a hub sci-
ence (Cacioppo, 2007), psychology is deeply
interconnected with other major sciences, such
as economics, education, and medicine. Intro
Psych has the potential for great influence be-
cause, as a gateway course, it disproportionately
serves college students in their first year, argued
to be a “sensitive” period of development (e.g.,
Blakemore & Mills, 2014; Brady, Hard, &
Gross, 2018). For many students, the first year
of college can be particularly challenging: Stu-
dents must adjust to a new, demanding environ-
ment in which they do not entirely know what to
expect. These features of the first-year experi-
ence mean that a student’s trajectory can be
shaped more dramatically during this time than
in later years of college. For example, social–
psychological interventions aimed at first-year
students, such as those targeting belonging, can
have lasting benefits, improving students’ aca-
demic performance and well-being years later
(Stephens, Townsend, Hamedani, Destin, &
Manzo, 2015; Walton & Cohen, 2007). Relat-
edly, first-year experience courses (e.g., “first-
year seminars”) are associated with students’
first-year GPA and retention in college (see
Permzadian & Credé, 2016, for a review), illus-
trating the potential for courses taken early in
college to have a lasting impact. Thus, like any
introductory course, Intro Psych has the poten-
tial to teach students foundational skills for later
coursework and shape their impressions of what
college is like. Intro Psych may even be an ideal
course to carry this hefty responsibility, given
that its content is directly relevant to academic
success (e.g., memory and learning), interper-
sonal success (e.g., social influence and cogni-
tion), and well-being (e.g., emotions, stress, and
mental health).
Current Understanding of Intro Psych’s
Impact on Students
And yet, we know little about whether and how
Intro Psych benefits students in the long term,
especially for those who take the course in their
first year of college. The existing research primar-
ily focuses on students’ acquisition of specific
content knowledge and suggests that lasting ef-
fects are less robust than instructors hope (see
Gurung & Hackathorn, 2018, for a review). While
students are more likely to remember concepts
and principles that were introduced with vivid,
novel instructional techniques, such as in-class
demonstrations (VanderStoep, Fagerlin, & Feen-
stra, 2000), both classic and recent research indi-
cates that students quickly forget course content
(Eurich, 1934; Greene, 1931; Gustav, 1969; Her-
man, 2010; Landrum & Gurung, 2013). For ex-
ample, in one study (Landrum & Gurung, 2013),
students performed with about 56% accuracy on a
comprehensive test of representative concepts
from their Intro Psych course, taken just two years
earlier. Even psychology majors enrolled in a cap-
stone course performed at a “D” level (63%) on
the test.
62 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Assessing how much content students retain
from Intro Psych is important, but so too is
assessing the broader skills and perspectives
that students may gain. Beyond specific facts
and definitions, what do students learn and take
with them, years after the course has ended? An
ideal way to assess the long-term impact of
taking Intro Psych in one’s first year of college
would be to randomly assign students to take
the course in their first year, perhaps through a
lottery approach, and then to assess their knowl-
edge and skills at various points during college
compared to waitlisted students who may or
may not take the course in a later year. Such an
effort would be most feasible at an institution
where Intro Psych is oversubscribed, and would
be strengthened with standardized assessments
of the knowledge and skills the course is in-
tended to teach. Researchers could examine
how experiences in the course predict perfor-
mance on those assessments both in the short
and long term. The American Psychological
Association is working to create standard as-
sessments of this nature (American Psycholog-
ical Association, 2017), but results using them
are likely years away. What can we learn in the
meantime using available research methods that
are relatively easy to implement?
The Present Study
The present study aims to extend previous
research on Intro Psych by addressing three
questions, outlined below. The measures for the
study were included in a broader research ef-
fort
1
that followed up with senior undergradu-
ates who had taken Intro Psych three years
earlier when they were first-year students.
First, what do students describe as the most
useful knowledge, skills, and perspectives
gained from their Intro Psych course, years after
course completion? In the survey, students were
asked to describe what from the course they
found most useful to them in college and in life
more broadly. Given previous findings that
memory for specific content fades quickly (e.g.,
Landrum & Gurung, 2013), we expected that
students would describe “big picture” insights
and perspectives rather than specific terms,
studies, or theories. We also examined whether
students who went on to major or minor in
psychology would describe different knowl-
edge, skills, and perspective than those who did
not.
Second, what do students actually retain from
Intro Psych in terms of specific content? Previ-
ous studies (Landrum & Gurung, 2013) have
approached this question by having former Intro
Psych students retake a cumulative exam with
items that were representative of the previous
exam they had taken. Students may or may not
have seen these the items when originally taking
the course. In the present study, we gave stu-
dents a subset of test items from their very first
exam in Intro Psych, which covered research
methods, the biological bases of behavior (i.e.,
genetics and evolution, the nervous system, and
brain), sensation and perception, and learning.
Students had all responded to these test items
previously, and we were able to compare their
prior performance as first-years on these items
to their current performance as seniors. Based
on previous findings, we expected that students
would forget considerable content in the years
since taking the course, and thus perform worse
on the retest than on the original. We also
expected that students who went on to special-
ize in psychology as majors or minors would
perform better on this retest than those who
didn’t, primarily due to repeated exposure to the
content in later courses.
Third, is having had a better experience in Intro
Psych as a first-year student (i.e., performed bet-
ter, enjoyed it more) associated with more adap-
tive academic behaviors in students’ senior year?
To address this question, we first focused on stu-
dents’ study strategies. In this particular course,
students attended lecture three times a week and
read 15 chapters in a 16-chapter textbook. There
was relatively little overlap between the content
covered in lectures and the content covered in the
textbook, so students were required to do consid-
erable self-study. In such an environment, stu-
dents had to quickly develop their study skills and
strategies. Based on the experiences of the course
coordinator (the first author) in teaching the course
for many years, we compiled a list of the most
common strategies that students reported using in
the course, and asked students about their ten-
1
In addition to the questions for this study, the broader
research effort also included measures to assess possible
long-term effects of a test anxiety intervention (Brady et al.,
2018).
63WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
dency to use these strategies years later, as seniors.
Next, we focused on students’ ability to manage
course projects. In their Intro Psych course, stu-
dents completed a project designed to give them
experience thinking like a psychologist—
specifically, students were required to propose an
original research idea, outline the methodology to
test it, and discuss implications of possible results.
The project was intentionally structured to model
adaptive project management approaches, in that
it had several different phases, included multiple
rounds of feedback, and required students to at-
tend office hours at least once. Students thus ex-
perienced the potential value of breaking large
projects into manageable steps, and seeking feed-
back from the instructor along the way. The grade
on the project was largely based on a final, revised
research proposal, and most of the students in our
sample performed extremely well (M 91.2,
SD 3.68, Range 80 to 99). We expected that
success with this experience might be related to
better project management skills later in college.
To find out, we asked students to rate their current
approach to course projects by responding to
questions that were inspired by the goals of the
Intro Psych research proposal project.
Method
Participants
We invited 237 senior undergraduate stu-
dents who had previously taken Intro Psych to
participate. These students had taken Intro
Psych during their first year (either Fall or Win-
ter quarter) at a selective, private university. Of
those invited, 156 (66%) completed the relevant
questions for this study.
2
Students who responded to the survey had
performed slightly better in the course (overall
grade 89.5, SD 5.21) than ones who did
not (M 87.4, SD 5.82), and this difference
was significant, t(234) ⫽⫺2.69, p .01, Co-
hen’s d 0.38. Although the sample overrep-
resented higher-performing students, the demo-
graphic composition of the respondents was
representative of the original sample. A slight
majority of the respondents were women
(63.5% compared to 58% in the original sample,
2
1.41, p .23), and about a quarter were
students from underrepresented racial–ethnic
minority backgrounds (27% compared to 29%
in the original sample,
2
.20, p .65).
Although we did not collect students’ ages,
most seniors at the university are 21 or 22 years
old.
Course and Study Design
The Intro Psych course that students took
during their first year offered lectures three
times a week, taught by a primary instructor. A
course coordinator oversaw the curriculum and
assessments. Students were required to attend
weekly discussion sections, led by one or two
teaching assistants who were a mix of psychol-
ogy graduate students and advanced undergrad-
uates. Students’ course grades were based on
three noncumulative exams in the course (60%),
a research proposal writing project (20%), an
in-class essay (10%), and participation in their
discussion sections (10%).
In the fall of their senior year, students re-
ceived an e-mail from the course instructor and
course coordinator, inviting them to complete a
30-min survey about their experiences in col-
lege in general and with the Intro Psych course
in particular. Procedures were approved by the
campus Institutional Review Board.
Students completed the survey online, via
Qualtrics. After completing the consent form,
students responded to a variety of questions,
discussed below.
3
Participants were compen-
sated with a $20 gift card.
Measures
All survey questions used in the study and all
relevant data to this article, with the exception
of student academic records, are available
through the Open Science Framework at https://
osf.io/tvgky/. A full data file, which includes
data from students’ academic records, is avail-
able from the first author.
Experience with, and reflections on,
psychology. Students responded to questions
about their experiences with psychology at the
university. They reported whether they were
majoring or minoring in psychology and how
2
Thirteen additional students partially completed the sur-
vey but did not respond to the questions that are the focus of
this study.
3
Students also completed other measures not related to
the current project, including questions about their level of
stress, their beliefs about stress, strategies for coping with
test anxiety, and their feelings of belonging.
64 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
many courses they had taken in psychology.
They responded to two open-ended prompts:
“What, if anything, did you learn in Intro
Psych
4
that has been useful to you in your other
classes?” and “What, if anything, did you learn
in Intro Psych that has been useful to you in
your life in general?”
Course content quiz. Students also com-
pleted a brief quiz that tested a subset of 16 test
items that they had completed during their first
midterm in the course.
5
The original midterm
contained 50 multiple choice items worth 75%
of the exam and a written answer section worth
25%. The 16 items encompassed all of the top-
ics assessed on the original midterm: research
methods, the biological bases of behavior (ge-
netics and evolution, the brain and nervous sys-
tem), sensation and perception, and learning.
Students’ original performance on these 16
items highly correlated with their overall per-
formance on the multiple-choice portion of the
midterm, r(155) .78, p .001, and modestly
correlated with their performance on the written
answer portion, r(155) .43, p .001.
Approaches to coursework during their se-
nior year. Students also answered questions
regarding their current approaches to course-
work. First, students rated how often they used
10 different study strategies, ranging from
memorizing definitions to attending faculty of-
fice hours in order to clarify material. Second,
students rated their approach to course projects
by responding to questions that were inspired by
the goals of the research proposal project they
had completed in Intro Psych. Students rated
whether, as seniors, they (1) focus on the larger
goals of the project and what they could learn
from it, (2) divide up projects into manageable
steps, (3) complete tasks a little bit at a time,
instead of at the last minute, and (4) reach out to
teaching staff for support and feedback.
Intro Psych course experience (assessed
three years previously). Students’ responses
to the online survey were de-identified and
linked to their prior data from the Intro Psych
course using a research ID number. Prior data
included the overall course grades as well as its
components: the average of three noncumula-
tive exams, a research proposal project that in-
volves proposing an original research study, an
in-class essay, and course participation.
Prior data also included responses to an on-
line course evaluation designed by the instruc-
tional team and delivered in class on the last day
of lecture. The evaluation asked students a wide
variety of questions about their course experi-
ences. Most relevant to the present research
were ratings of how interesting lectures were
and the overall effectiveness of the primary
instructor, the course coordinator, and the stu-
dent’s section teaching assistant (TA; one item
each). The ratings for the primary instructor and
the course coordinator were moderately corre-
lated, r(148) .49, p .001, which was sen-
sible given that they were perceived by students
as co-leading the class. Thus, we averaged these
two ratings together into a single average for the
instructor.
Results
What Do Students Say They Learned From
Intro Psych?
Our first research question was, what do se-
nior students identify as the most useful knowl-
edge, skills, and perspectives gained from their
Intro Psych course their first year? We used
thematic analysis—coding for major thematic
categories—to analyze students’ qualitative re-
sponses to two questions: (1) What did you
learn in Intro Psych that has been useful to you
in your other classes? (2) What did you learn in
Intro Psych that has been useful to you in your
life in general? Using an inductive approach,
the second author read the entire corpus of
responses and then generated a list of themes
that were mentioned by a significant number of
students (i.e., more than 10% of responses).
Then, the second author created a codebook to
quantify the number of responses that fit a set of
criteria for each major theme. An additional
coder also coded the responses to determine
interrater reliability, which was adequate, with
Cohen’s ranging from 0.66 to 0.83.
4
In the survey, the Intro Psych course was referenced by
its institutional course number. Here, and in later examples
of student responses, we have replaced this course number
with “Intro Psych.”
5
As mentioned previously, this project capitalized on
another research effort to assess the long-term impacts of an
anxiety reappraisal intervention (Brady et al., 2018). The 16
test items were not randomly selected, but were chosen
based on which test items had shown the largest effects of
the intervention when students were previously enrolled in
the course.
65WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Thematic analysis revealed two major themes
in students’ descriptions of what they learned
that was useful in other classes: first, the devel-
opment of study skills and, second, founda-
tional knowledge for subsequent classes. See
Figure 1.
About a fifth (21.2%) of students said the
course helped them identify and hone study
skills useful in other college classes. Of these,
some students described learning in Intro Psych
that applying concepts to their lives was an
effective way to learn course material. Others
reported that course content helped them iden-
tify effective strategies such as self-quizzing
and studying in environments that were similar
to the environment they would be tested in.
Illustrative responses include the following:
From [Intro Psych], I have learned that I can best grasp
material in classes if I try to envision how I can apply
concepts to the world around me.
It taught me to study hard to do well in classes. I take
more time to study with others in test-like environ-
ments and to quiz myself as much as possible, drawing
on old information.
Another fifth of students (21.8%) said the
course equipped them with foundational knowl-
edge for future classes. Many students men-
tioned that the material covered in course was
revisited in greater depth in their later psychol-
ogy courses. In addition, some students reported
the course was helpful in nonpsychology
courses, such as in human biology or product
design. For example,
[Intro Psych] does a really good job at previewing
course content for other classes. Almost every psych
class I’ve taken after [Intro Psych] (if not all) brought
up a topic that was touched upon on [Intro Psych]. The
classes elaborate upon what I was introduced to in
[Intro Psych].
It was helpful to know about the different functions of
the parts of the brain for my later human biology
classes.
Some students (9.6%) mentioned specific
concepts and ideas they had found memorable
and useful, but these did not converge on any
single themes. For example, several students
mentioned growth mindset (Dweck, 2006), ste-
reotype threat (Steele, 1997), and memorization
techniques (e.g., semantic encoding; Hyde &
Jenkins, 1973) as important concepts that they
learned. A larger number of students (34%)
described learning things that seemed to capture
22% 35% 22% 22%21% 16% 23% 11%
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
30%
35%
40%
45%
50%
Study Skills Foundational Knowledge Understanding Others Interacting with Others
Percentage of Students Mentioning Theme
Psych Students (N = 23)
Non-Psych Students (N = 133)
Figure 1. Students’ qualitative descriptions of what they learned from Intro Psych showed
highly similar themes regardless of whether the students specialized (i.e., majored or minored)
in psychology or not. The only exception was that psychology students were more likely to
report that the course provided foundational knowledge that was useful in later courses. See
the online article for the color version of this figure.
66 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
the overarching content of the course, but were
too vague to be thematic, such as “the way the
mind works.”
Did students who specialized in psychology
as an academic major or minor report learning
different knowledge and skills than those stu-
dents who did not? Although the majority of the
students did not specialize in psychology, 14%
did (N
majors
17; N
minors
6). We found that
themes in students’ responses differed some-
what as a function of whether they specialized
in psychology or not. As shown in Figure 1,
psychology students were more likely to report
that the class equipped them with foundational
knowledge for future classes,
2
4.67, p
.03. No other differences were observed.
Thematic analysis revealed two major themes
in students’ response to the second question
regarding what they learned in Intro Psych that
was useful to their lives in general. Both themes
emphasized the social relevance of the course.
A quarter of students (25%) reported that the
knowledge gained in the course helped them
develop a deeper understanding of people, such
as an appreciation for how situations shape peo-
ple’s behavior, and greater empathy for others.
Students said the following:
I think it shifted my perspective on how I viewed
people I previously didn’t understand, and it helped me
understand how each of us can act differently in the
same situations.
[Intro Psych] has been helpful in helping me to be
more empathetic toward people and myself. It helps me
to think critically about how or why people are who
they are.
Relatedly, students described how the knowl-
edge they gained in the course helped them
interact better with others (12.8%). Students
wrote the following:
I think [Intro Psych] helped me challenge my imme-
diate judgements and think critically about the way I
and others think. It helps when thinking about how
people perceive things and interacting with others.
Studying emotion regulation and understanding how
people process emotions has helped in interacting with
people in healthy ways.
As with the other open-ended question, some
students (10.3%) mentioned specific concepts and
ideas they had found useful, but these did not
converge on a specific theme. One student, for
example, listed “growth mindset, bystander apa-
thy, Pygmalion effect.” A larger number of stu-
dents (35.9%) gave ambiguous responses that
seemed to describe the broad content of Intro
Psych, such as one student who said the course
helped them recognize “different behavioral phe-
nomenon[a] that occur.” There were no differ-
ences in the types of responses to this question as
a function of whether students specialized in psy-
chology, largest
2
1.92, p .17 (see Figure 1).
How Much do Students Actually Remember?
How well did students remember the course
content, as measured by the 16-item quiz on ques-
tions they had previously completed during their
first exam in Intro Psych? Did students who spe-
cialized in psychology retain the content better
than students who did not? We performed a 2 2
mixed design ANOVA with timepoint as a within-
subjects factor (first year, senior year) and aca-
demic specialty (psychology students, nonpsy-
chology students) as a between-subjects factor. As
shown in Figure 2, students answered 89% of
these questions correctly when they were enrolled
in the course as first-years (SD 9.9%). As se-
niors, they answered significantly fewer questions
correctly (M 70%, SD 17.2%), F(1, 153)
62.42, p .001, M
diff
18.3%, 95% CI [15.6%,
21.0%],
2
.12. Whether students specialized in
psychology significantly predicted performance,
F(1, 153) 8.27, p .01,
2
.03, but this
effect interacted with timepoint, F(1, 153) 7.73,
p .01,
2
.02. In follow-up analyses, we
found that psychology students did not differ from
nonpsychology students in their scores on these
items as first-year students, t(153) 1, p .38,
M
diff
2.0%, 95% CI [2.5%, 6.4%], d ⫽⫺0.2.
However, psychology students performed better
on the items as seniors, answering significantly
more items correctly (M 81%, SD 14.3%)
than nonpsychology students (M 69%, SD
17.0%), t(154) 3.26, p .001, M
diff
12.3%,
95% CI [4.8%, 19.7%], d 0.74.
The superior quiz performance of the psychol-
ogy students is likely driven by the higher number
of additional psychology courses they took (M
8.00, SD 2.76) compared to nonpsychology
students (M 1.39, SD 1.62), t(154) 16.06,
p .001, M
diff
6.6, 95% CI [5.8, 7.4], d 3.63.
Nonpsychology students who took no other psy-
chology courses after Intro Psych (N 48) an-
swered 66% of items correctly (SD 17%).
We tested whether the number of additional
psychology courses accounted for the effects of
67WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
specializing in psychology on quiz performance
using a hierarchical linear regression analysis. At
Step 1, academic specialty (psychology stu-
dents 1, nonpsychology students 0) was
entered as the sole predictor of quiz performance
and was significant, ␤⫽.123, 95% CI [.048,
.197], p .001. Alone, academic specialty ac-
counted for 6.5% of the variation in quiz perfor-
mance. At Step 2, we added the number of psy-
chology courses taken to the model, which
explained 4.5% additional variance in quiz perfor-
mance, and this change in R
2
was significant, F(1,
253) 7.66, p .02. Critically, when number of
psychology courses was included in the model, it
was a significant predictor of quiz performance,
␤⫽.020, 95% CI [.006, .034], p .01, but
academic specialty was not, ␤⫽⫺.009, 95% CI
[.128, .110], p .88. This provides strong ev-
idence that psychology students performed better
on the quiz because of their additional coursework
in psychology.
Predictably, first-year performance on the
items predicted senior-year performance,
r(155) .33, p .001, 95% CI [.18, .46].
Students who answered more questions cor-
rectly while taking the course answered more
questions correctly several years later.
Does Experience in Intro Psych Predict
Later Academic Behaviors?
Given that many students reported that the
course helped them to develop academic skills
for other courses, was there any evidence that
better experiences in the course as first-years
predicted more effective approaches to their
college coursework in their senior year, such as
greater use of study strategies and more adap-
tive project management skills?
6
Table 1 pro-
vides descriptive statistics for students’ initial
performance in the course, as well as their rat-
ings on course evaluations. Table 2 provides
descriptive statistics for each item measuring
students’ study strategies and project manage-
ment skills, as well as the averages across items.
6
Degrees of freedom vary somewhat across analyses due
to missing data from when students originally took the
course.
90% 81%89% 69%
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
100%
First-Year (enrolled in course) Senior Year (follow up)
Performance on 16-item Quiz (% Correct)
Psych Students (N = 23) Non-Psych Students (N = 133)
Figure 2. Overall, students performed less well on a subset of 16 exam items as seniors than
they had on those same items when enrolled in Intro Psych as first-year students. Students who
specialized in psychology (i.e., majored or minored) did not perform differently on the test
items than nonpsychology students when enrolled in Intro Psych their first year of college.
They did perform better as seniors on the same test items, however. Error bars show 1 SEM.
See the online article for the color version of this figure.
68 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Neither students’ overall Intro Psych course
grade, nor their grades specifically on exams, pre-
dicted their use of study strategies—either overall,
r(154) ⫽⫺.023, p .78, or for any specific study
strategy, smallest p .134. Did students’ enjoy-
ment of the course, specifically their ratings of the
course on the end-of-term evaluation, predict later
study strategies? We found that overall ratings of
the course instructors did predict overall use of
study strategies, r(148) .266, p .001, 95% CI
[.12, .42], and more specifically predicted the
strategies of quizzing oneself, r(148) .219, p
.007, 95% CI [.06, .37], and attending office hours
to ask about confusing material, r(148) .174,
p .03, 95% CI [.01, .33]. Ratings of how inter-
esting lectures were, of their section TA, and of
other features of the course did not predict study
strategies.
What about students’ approaches to course
projects? Overall course grades in Intro Psych
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Course Performance and Evaluations
M [95% CI] SD N
Course performance in Intro Psych during first year
Course Grade 89.59 [88.76, 90.43] 5.24 154
Average Exam Score 87.83 [86.65, 89.01] 7.45 156
Research Project Grade 91.20 [90.62, 91.78] 3.68 155
Section Participation Grade 94.06 [93.19, 94.93] 5.46 155
Evaluation ratings of Intro Psych during first year
Lectures Overall: Satisfaction with the “interestingness and engagingness” of lectures
(scale: 1 very dissatisfied to 5 very satisfied) 4.72 .48 148
Instructor Rating: Average overall effectiveness rating for the instructor and course
coordinator (scale: 1 poor to 5 excellent) 4.77 [4.71, 4.83] .38 148
Overall Effectiveness of Teaching Assistant (scale: 1 poor to 5 excellent) 4.27 [4.15, 4.39] .73 148
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for Study Strategies and Project Management Skills
STUDY STRATEGIES
How much do you use each of the following study strategies before exams at [Institution Name]?
(1 Never,2 Sometimes,3 Most of the time,4 Always)
MSDN
Memorizing definitions 2.36 [2.23, 2.49] .80 156
Re-reading the textbook 2.17 [2.05, 2.30] .78 156
Reviewing lecture slides 3.38 [3.27, 3.49] .71 146
Reviewing my own notes 3.30 [3.17, 3.43] .84 156
Rewriting notes 2.06 [1.89, 2.23] 1.07 156
Creating mnemonics 1.78 [1.66, 1.90] .76 156
Studying with friends 2.14 [2.01, 2.27] .82 156
Quizzing myself 2.65 [2.51, 2.79] .90 156
Reviewing prior assignments or problem sets 3.13 [2.99, 3.26] .86 156
Attending office hours to ask about confusing material 2.32 [2.19, 2.45] .84 156
Average: Overall use of study strategies 2.53 [2.47, 2.59] .38 156
PROJECT MANAGEMENT SKILLS
Please rate your agreement with each of the following statements:
When working on a course project...(1 Strongly disagree,7 Strongly agree)
...Ithink about the overall goal of the project and what I am supposed to learn from it. 4.74 [4.52, 4.95] 1.37 156
...Itrytobreak the project into smaller, more manageable steps. 5.49 [5.30, 5.67] 1.18 156
...Idoalittle but at a time, rather than putting off tasks to the last minute. 4.11 [3.85, 4.36] 1.62 156
...Ifeel comfortable reaching out to my instructor or TA for support and feedback 5.08 [4.85, 5.30] 1.43 156
Average: Overall project management skills [4.71, 5.00] 156
69WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
did predict adaptive project management skills
during the senior year, r(154) .24, p .003,
95% CI [.08, .38]. As shown in Table 3, all
aspects of course grade correlated with later
approach to projects, but students’ grade on the
research proposal project was the mostly
strongly correlated, r(154) .32, p .001,
95% CI [.17, .45]. Ratings of the course instruc-
tors also predicted more adaptive project man-
agement skills, r(148) .294, p .001, 95%
CI [.14, .44], but ratings of the section TA (who
most directly mentored students on the project)
did not, r(148) ⫽⫺.03, p .76.
Discussion
The present study represents a first step to-
ward understanding the influence of Intro Psych
by surveying senior undergraduates who com-
pleted the course in their first year of college.
We aimed to generate insight into what students
think they learned, how much detail they actu-
ally retained, and whether their experiences in
the course as first-years predicted any of their
academic behaviors as seniors. Although the
study has a number of limitations, it provides
initial evidence of the lasting impact that Intro
Psych may have on students, hopefully inspir-
ing additional, more rigorous research efforts.
Students Report Learning Skills,
Knowledge, and Perspectives
Years after the course, students described
how Intro Psych shaped their skills, knowledge,
and perspectives. Students reported that the
class fostered effective study strategies and
foundational knowledge that they applied to
other courses. More broadly, students said the
course shaped their perspectives on the social
world by helping them understand and interact
more effectively with others. By and large, stu-
dents who went on to specialize in psychology
reported gaining similar ideas and skills when
compared to nonmajors.
This similarity between majors and nonma-
jors may reflect that their Intro Psych course
was designed to serve majors and nonmajors in
similar ways, as has been recommended by the
American Psychological Association and by
prominent scholars in the scholarship of teach-
ing and learning within psychology (Gurung et
al., 2016; Halpern, 2010). Alternatively, majors
and nonmajors may report learning similar con-
cepts and skills because they tend to have sim-
ilar interests in an Intro Psych course. Prior
work suggests that students in Intro Psych, re-
gardless of their intended major, tend to value
concepts that are relevant to their immediate
needs as students, such as academic and inter-
personal success. Zanich and Grover (1989)
found, in their surveys of students just before
and after taking Intro Psych, that students grav-
itate toward these topics regardless of their ma-
jor, and remain similarly interested in these
topics after the course is over. Our findings hint
that these interests are reflected in what is re-
tained years after the course has ended.
Content Knowledge Lingers, Especially
for Majors
In comparison to previous studies, we found
evidence of relatively strong retention of course
concepts. We retested students on concepts
originally presented on their first midterm and
Table 3
Correlations Among Course Performance, Evaluations, and Outcome Measures
1 2 3 4 5 6789
1. Course Grade
2. Average Exam Score .981
ⴱⴱⴱ
3. Research Project Grade .544
ⴱⴱⴱ
.420
ⴱⴱⴱ
4. Section Participation Grade .450
ⴱⴱⴱ
.356
ⴱⴱⴱ
.284
ⴱⴱⴱ
5. Rating of Lectures Overall .009 .020 .024 .026
6. Rating of Instructor Effectiveness .131 .109 .145 .138 .166
7. Rating of TA Effectiveness .050 .070 .106 .030 .053 .144
8. Study Strategies .009 .033 .048 .120 .023 .266
ⴱⴱ
.032
9. Project Management Skills .239
ⴱⴱ
.201
.316
ⴱⴱⴱ
.234
ⴱⴱ
.075 .294
ⴱⴱⴱ
.025 .382
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p .001.
70 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
compared their performance across the two
tests. Students experienced a 21% drop in per-
formance compared to their performance as
first-year students, but still earned what would
have been a passing grade of 71%—meaning-
fully higher than the 56%–63% performance
rates found in other studies (Landrum & Gu-
rung, 2013). Still, and consistent with prior
findings, students performed better on the retest
if they were majoring or minoring in psychol-
ogy. Nonmajors performed better if they had
taken more additional courses in psychology,
and students whose only psychology course was
Intro Psych scored about 65%.
There are several possible explanations for
why students showed greater evidence of reten-
tion than students in prior work. One explana-
tion relates to the nature of the sample: Students
in the present study attended a highly selective
university and likely had strong study skills that
boosted long-term learning. Additionally, high-
er-performing students were more likely to re-
spond to the survey, a self-selection bias not
found in previous studies (Landrum & Gurung,
2013). Other explanations relate to the nature of
the first exam in an Intro Psych course. Early
parts of an Intro Psych course tend to cover
foundational content (e.g., research methods,
biological bases) that is likely to be revisited
during later parts of the course and potentially
in other courses inside and outside of psychol-
ogy. Additionally, the very first exam in a
course, especially during a student’s first year,
may be particularly meaningful and memorable.
Students likely study harder for this exam be-
cause they do not know what to expect.
Course Performance and Evaluations
Predict Some Academic Behaviors
Additionally, several aspects of students’ ac-
ademic behavior as seniors were correlated with
their prior experiences in Intro Psych. Specifi-
cally, first-year students who performed better
on a term project later self-reported more use of
effective project management skills as seniors.
This relationship hints at the potential for intro-
ductory course assignments to shape students’
approach to assignments in later courses. We
interpret this correlational finding cautiously,
however, just as we would teach our students to
do. We do not know whether students who
engaged well with the project (and thus earned
a good grade) developed effective project man-
agement skills, or whether students who already
had effective project management skills in their
first year (and thus still had them in their senior
year) engaged well with the project. Addition-
ally, our measure of project management skills
is novel and quite brief. Further work is needed
to determine its reliability and validity or to
develop a more detailed and robust measure.
Of note, students’ performance in the course
was largely unrelated to their later use of study
strategies. Our measure of study strategies was
coarse and self-reported—we simply asked stu-
dents to indicate their use of a range of different
study strategies and did not capture individual
differences in studying efficiency or whether
students adopted the best strategy for a given
context. Future work should make use of vali-
dated measures of study strategies, such as the
Study Behavior Checklist (Gurung, Weidert, &
Jeske, 2010) or the Ten Learning Techniques
(Bartoszewski & Gurung, 2015). Furthermore,
because our sample comprised students at a
highly selective university who are likely rela-
tively skilled at studying, even as first-year stu-
dents, the range in use of study strategies may
have been too restricted to detect a relationship
with prior grades.
Although we found a modest relationship be-
tween how much students enjoyed the course,
as measured by their ratings on course evalua-
tions, and their later outcomes, these relation-
ships were not especially revealing. We found,
for example, that students’ ratings of the course
instructors were related to using more study
strategies, on average, and with using a partic-
ularly effective study strategy: self-quizzing
(e.g., Roediger, Agarwal, McDaniel, & Mc-
Dermott, 2011). Students’ ratings of the course
instructors were also correlated with their later
project management skills. Although it is pos-
sible that students were inspired by their in-
structors to develop these skills, it is equally
plausible that students who mastered studying
and tackling course projects in their first-year
Intro Psych course also came to like their in-
structors more. There may also be a third vari-
able: Students who were more academically
engaged in their first year both liked their in-
structors more and developed more effective
academic strategies.
There are many reasons why responses to
course evaluations may be unrelated to later
71WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
outcomes. In particular, some work suggests
that course evaluations do not capture the true
quality of students’ learning experience. They
are influenced less by how much students learn
than by course characteristics, such as class
size, and instructor characteristics, such as gen-
der and academic rank (Johnson, Narayanan, &
Sawaya, 2013).
Limitations and Future Directions
Our study is one of only a few to follow up
with students years after they have taken Intro
Psych and provides initial evidence that Intro
Psych teaches students much more than terms
and definitions. Although the present findings
provide a promising first look at the impact of
Intro Psych, much remains unknown. First, how
does the long-term impact of Intro Psych de-
pend on the design of the course and the char-
acteristics of its students? Our sample was lim-
ited to students at a selective private institution
with a well-funded, highly supportive Intro
Psych course. Students in this course performed
very well on average and rated the course in-
structors very positively. Our findings were also
limited by self-selection bias—students who re-
sponded to our survey were higher-performing
than those who did not. Thus, our findings may
describe the “best case scenario” for Intro
Psych: what is possible when well-prepared stu-
dents take an Intro Psych course designed to
support their long-term learning. Whether and
how these findings extend to different popula-
tions of students experiencing different Intro
Psych courses is a question for future research.
Second, how can we learn about the broader
ideas and skills that students gain from the
course without relying on self-report? Students
may not be fully aware of, or able to articulate,
the broader ideas and skills they gain from a
course, highlighting a key need for objective
follow-up assessments. For example, a vast ma-
jority of students did not mention the class
cultivating critical thinking skills, a key learn-
ing goal for many Intro Psych courses. Students
may not understand or be able to adequately
self-assess their own critical thinking. Future
work may look to include standardized assess-
ments of critical thinking skills.
A third salient question is whether and to
what extent introductory courses in other disci-
plines predict student outcomes. Are there gen-
eral academic skills and orientations that any
well-developed introductory course, regardless
of discipline, could help foster? We found that
experiences in Intro Psych during the first year
predicted approaches to coursework years later.
Would performance in any of a students’ intro
courses, especially those taken during the first
year, be similarly predictive? If so, are such
courses directly developing those academic be-
haviors? There is little data to address this ques-
tion. Although researchers have explored the
long-term impact of specific coursework, such
as the influence of taking college economics
classes on future civic behaviors (Allgood,
Bosshardt, van der Klaauw, & Watts, 2004),
this work does not focus on courses taken in the
first year of college. Research on the impact of
first-year experience courses, largely seminars,
finds that taking these courses is associated with
better student achievement and persistence (see
Permzadian & Credé, 2016, for a review), but
does not address the development of students’
academic skills. To our knowledge, no existing
studies examine the long-term effect of disci-
plinary introductory courses taken in the first
year. Future studies could obtain data from
other first-year courses to examine discipline-
specific impacts. Understanding how and why
different first-year courses shape later outcomes
could guide institutions in making better curric-
ulum decisions and students in selecting first
year courses.
Finally, how does Intro Psych influence stu-
dents with different interests and goals? We
found that students described similar “lessons
learned” from the course regardless of whether
they were majors or nonmajors, but we do not
yet know whether students carry slightly differ-
ent skills and perspectives with them depending
on their chosen major or career (e.g., health-
related professions or business-related profes-
sions).
Concluding Remarks
Intro Psych serves many students and has the
potential to cultivate valuable skills and ideas
that students can carry with them well beyond
the final exam. Using both qualitative and quan-
titative data, the current research takes a critical
first step to understanding the long-term impact
of the course on students’ skills, broader per-
spectives, and experiences. Given the impor-
72 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
tance of Intro Psych both to our field and, ar-
guably, to a liberal arts education, we hope
these findings inspire new efforts to clarify and
expand our knowledge of this course.
References
Allgood, S., Bosshardt, W., van der Klaauw, W., &
Watts, M. (2004). What students remember and
say about college economics years later. American
Economic Review, 94, 259–265. Retrieved from
https://econpapers.repec.org/article/aeaaecrev/
v_3a94_3ay_3a2004_3ai_3a2_3ap_3a259-265
.htm
American Psychological Association. (2014).
Strengthening the common core of the introductory
psychology course. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association, Board of Educational
Affairs. Retrieved from www.apa.org/ed/gover
nance/bea/intro-psych-report.pdf
American Psychological Association. (2017). Assess-
ment of outcomes of the introductory course in
psychology. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association, Board of Educational Affairs.
Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/ed/precollege/
assessment-outcomes.pdf
Bartoszewski, B. L., & Gurung, R. A. R. (2015).
Comparing the relationship of learning techniques
and exam score. Scholarship of Teaching and
Learning in Psychology, 1, 219–228. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/stl0000036
Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adoles-
cence a sensitive period for sociocultural process-
ing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–207.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-
115202
Brady, S. T., Hard, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2018).
Reappraising test anxiety increases academic per-
formance of first-year college students. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 110, 395–406. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000219
Cacioppo, J. (2007, September). Psychology is a hub
science. APS Observer, 20. Retrieved from https://
www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/psycholo
gy-is-a-hub-science
Clay, R. (2017, November). Trends report: Psychology
is more popular than ever. Monitor on Psychology,
48. Retrieved from http://apa.org/monitor/
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology
of success. New York, NY: Random House.
Eurich, A. C. (1934). Retention of knowledge ac-
quired in a course in general psychology. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 18, 209–219. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/h0071566
Greene, E. B. (1931). The retention of information
learned in college courses. The Journal of Educa-
tional Research, 24, 262–273. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/00220671.1931.10880208
Gurung, R. A., & Hackathorn, J. (2018). Ramp it up:
A call for more research in introductory psychol-
ogy. Teaching of Psychology, 45, 302–311. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0098628318796413
Gurung, R. A., Hackathorn, J., Enns, C., Frantz, S.,
Cacioppo, J. T., Loop, T., & Freeman, J. E. (2016).
Strengthening introductory psychology: A new
model for teaching the introductory course. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 71, 112–124. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0040012
Gurung, R. A. R., Weidert, J., & Jeske, A. (2010).
Focusing on how students study. The Journal of
Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 10, 28–35.
Gustav, A. (1969). Retention of course material after
varying intervals of time. Psychological Reports,
25, 727–730. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1969
.25.3.727
Halpern, D. F. (Ed.). (2010). Undergraduate educa-
tion in psychology: A blueprint for the future of the
discipline. Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
ical Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12063-
000
Herman, W. E. (2010, March). How much do stu-
dents
remember
from an introductory psychology
course? Paper presented at the Farmingdale State
College Annual Conference on the Teaching of
Psychology: Ideas & Innovations, Tarrytown, NY.
Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1973). Recall for words
as a function of semantic, graphic, and syntactic
orienting tasks. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 12, 471–480. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80027-1
Johnson, M. D., Narayanan, A., & Sawaya, W. J.
(2013). Effects of course and instructor characteris-
tics on student evaluation of teaching across a college
of engineering. Journal of Engineering Education,
102, 289–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jee.20013
Landrum, R. (2017). A skills theme for the introduc-
tory psychology course. In D. Dunn & B. M. Hard
(Eds.), Thematic approaches for teaching intro-
ductory psychology (pp. 130–139). Boston, MA:
Cengage Learning.
Landrum, R. E., & Gurung, R. A. (2013). The mem-
orability of introductory psychology revisited.
Teaching of Psychology, 40, 222–227. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1177/0098628313487417
Mitchell, K., Lewis, R. S., Satterfield, J., & Hong,
B. A. (2016). The new Medical College Admission
Test: Implications for teaching psychology. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 71, 125–135. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0039975
Nord, C., Roey, S., Perkins, R., Lyons, M., Leman-
ski, N., Brown, J., & Schuknecht, J. (2011). Ame-
rica’s high school graduates: Results of the 2009
NAEP High School Transcript Study (NCES
2011–462). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
73WHAT STUDENTS REMEMBER ABOUT INTRO PSYCH
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Education, National Center for Education Statis-
tics. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/
pubsinfo.asp?pubid2011462
Permzadian, V., & Credé, M. (2016). Do first-year
seminars improve college grades and retention? A
quantitative review of their overall effectiveness
and an examination of moderators of effectiveness.
Review of Educational Research, 86, 277–316.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654315584955
Roediger, H. L., III, Agarwal, P. K., McDaniel,
M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2011). Test-enhanced
learning in the classroom: Long-term improve-
ments from quizzing. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Applied, 17, 382–395. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0026252
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How ste-
reotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor-
mance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
Stephens, N. M., Townsend, S. S. M., Hamedani,
M. G., Destin, M., & Manzo, V. (2015). A differ-
ence-education intervention equips first-generation
college students to thrive in the face of stressful
college situations. Psychological Science, 26,
1556–1566. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/09567976
15593501
Strohmetz, D. B., Dolinsky, B., Jhangiani, R. S.,
Posey, D. C., Hardin, E. E., Shyu, V., & Klein, E.
(2015). The skillful major: Psychology curricula in
the 21st Century. Scholarship of Teaching and
Learning in Psychology, 1, 200–207. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/stl0000037
VanderStoep, S. W., Fagerlin, A., & Feenstra, J. S.
(2000). What do students remember from intro-
ductory psychology? Teaching of Psychology, 27,
89–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328023TOP
2702_02
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of
belonging: Race, social fit, and achievement. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 82–
96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.82
Zanich, M. L., & Grover, D. E. (1989). Introductory
psychology from the standpoint of the consumer.
Teaching of Psychology, 16, 72–74. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1207/s15328023top1602_7
Received September 16, 2018
Revision received November 18, 2018
Accepted November 20, 2018
74 HARD, LOVETT, AND BRADY
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.