Cross-Cultural Industrial Organizational Psychology and Organizational
Behavior: A Hundred-Year Journey
Michele J. Gelfand
University of Maryland, College Park
Zeynep Aycan
Koç University
Miriam Erez
Technion - Israel Institute of Technology
Kwok Leung
Chinese University of Hong Kong
In celebration of the anniversary of the Journal of Applied Psychology (JAP), we take a hundred-
year journey to examine how the science of cross-cultural industrial/organizational psychology and
organizational behavior (CCIO/OB) has evolved, both in JAP and in the larger field. We review
broad trends and provide illustrative examples in the theoretical, methodological, and analytic
advances in CCIO/OB during 4 main periods: the early years (1917–1949), the middle 20th century
(1950 –1979), the later 20th century (1980 –2000), and the 21st century (2000 to the present). Within
each period, we discuss key historical and societal events that influenced the development of the
science of CCIO/OB, major trends in research on CCIO/OB in the field in general and JAP in
particular, and important milestones and breakthroughs achieved. We highlight pitfalls in research
on CCIO/OB and opportunities for growth. We conclude with recommendations for the next 100
years of CC IO/OB research in JAP and beyond.
Keywords: cross-cultural, industrial-organizational, organizational behavior
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000186.supp
Cross-cultural industrial and organizational psychology
and organizational behavior (CCIO/OB) has a long past and a
short history. Although many of the theories of work behavior
have been developed in the last 100 years, starting with the
launching of the Journal of Applied Psychology (JAP) in 1917,
questions of how to best manage behavior in organizations were
discussed around the globe for centuries prior to the formaliza-
tion of the field. For example, effective selection procedures
were featured in the Old Testament, in which God advised
Gideon on whom to choose for battle (Judges 7:4 New Revised
Standard Version), and the first systematic employee selection
system (i.e., the “imperial examination”) was developed in
China as early as the Han Dynasty. Likewise, in The Republic,
Plato discussed person–job fit, advocating that the ideal job was
one in which the person’s nature is “fitted for the task” (Plato
& Jowett, 1901, p. 56, cited in Antonakis, 2011). Lay theories
of group processes and leadership can also be found in the Bible
and other ancient texts, and advice on persuasion and influence
figures prominently in Sun Tzu’s Art of War (Tzu, 1963). It is
clear that the quest to understand work behavior has been a
global concern since antiquity.
In celebration of the anniversary of JAP, we take a hundred-
year journey to examine how the science of CCIO/OB has
evolved, both in JAP and in the larger field. Our review is
selective by necessity. We review broad trends and provide
illustrative examples in the theoretical, methodological, and
analytic advances in CCIO/OB during four main periods: the
early years (1917–1949), the middle 20th century (1950–1979),
the later 20th century (1980 –2000), and the 21st century (2000
to present). Within each period, we discuss key historical
and societal events that influenced the development of the
science of CCIO/OB, major trends in research on CCIO/OB in
the field in general, and JAP in particular, and important
milestones and breakthroughs. To track the evolution of the
field, we coded all articles in JAP during the period of 1917 to
2014 that focused explicitly on culture and organizational
phenomena, and we traced the evolution of the field of
This article was published Online First February 16, 2017.
Michele J. Gelfand, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland,
College Park; Zeynep Aycan, Department of Psychology and Department
of Business Administration, Koç University; Miriam Erez, Faculty of
Industrial Engineering & Management, Technion - Israel Institute of Tech-
nology; Kwok Leung, Department of Management, Chinese University of
Hong Kong.
This article is dedicated to our dear friend and colleague Kwok Leung,
who passed away during the writing of this article. The authors thank
Joshua C. Jackson, Christina Fahmi, and Nava Caluori for their help in this
review.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michele
J. Gelfand, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Bio-
Psychology Building, College Park, MD 20742. E-mail: mjgelfand@gmail
.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Applied Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 102, No. 3, 514–529 0021-9010/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000186
514
CCIO/OB in JAP on the following criteria: content of the
research (e.g., selection, training, attitudes, personality, intelli-
gence, conflict, teams, and leadership, among others); method-
ology (e.g., laboratory, field); whether aspects of culture (e.g.,
values, norms) were empirically assessed; whether measure-
ment equivalence was assessed; whether culture was examined
as a main effect or if was considered as a moderator (e.g.,
interacts with other contextual factors or individual differ-
ences); and whether the research focused on intracultural or
intercultural comparisons.
1
Early Years: 1917–1949
The discipline of I/O psychology began to develop during the
late 1800s and early 1900s in Europe and the United States. During
this era, Taylor’s (1914) principles of scientific management were
influential amid rapid industrialization and mass manufacturing,
and the zeitgeist was marked by a quest for the discovery of
general principles that were applicable across situations and peo-
ple. The United States had seen a large influx of immigrants from
different countries since the beginning of the 20th century, but the
rise in cultural diversity had not yet drawn attention to the impor-
tance of studying cultural influences on organizational phenome-
non. This was, in large part, because of a focus on minimizing
cultural differences. Indeed, Frost (1920) advocated that a major
contribution of industrial psychologists was the “Americanization
of the alien,” that is, the assimilation of individuals from different
nations, with different attitudes, values, and behaviors, into U.S.
organizations. This view was consistent with the melting pot view
of culture prominent in the United States. In Europe, William
Wundt and his students conducted some of the first research
related to I/O psychology. However, these early studies did not
examine how culture affects work behavior, and it was only in his
later publication of Völkerpsychologie that Wundt (1906) advo-
cated that culture plays a critical role in understanding human
mind and behavior (Araujo, 2013). Yet despite Wundt’s efforts,
this work did not have much influence on psychological research
during his lifetime, and did not lead to a focus on culture in IO/OB.
Later during this period, World War I (1914–1918) precipitated
the need to screen recruits for various military roles, causing
individual differences such as intelligence to figure prominently in
this period’s research (Vinchur & Koppes, 2011). In the United
States, testing and selection researchers working for the Army
were aware of the influence of culture, but the focus was on
creating a selection test for those with lower English-language
skills (the Army Beta). In World War II (1939 –1945), even more
attention was given to the selection and placement of soldiers, but
culture was still ignored (Katzell & Austin, 1992). In one excep-
tion, Stouffer and his colleagues studied American soldiers in
World War II and analyzed the perception of relative deprivation
among soldiers of different ethnicities (Stouffer, Suchman, DeVin-
ney, Star, & Williams, 1949). This study provided an important
conceptual basis for the development of equity theory, a major
theory in I/O psychology (Adams, 1965). It is fair to conclude that
I/O psychology in the very early years in the United States re-
mained largely culture bound, that is, theories were tested only on
American samples, and culture blind, that is, culture was not
considered as an important factor (Katzell & Austin, 1992).
Several other notable events occurred during this period that
warrant discussion. In his cultural analysis of economic systems,
Max Weber (1905/1958) discussed the Protestant work ethic and
the rise of capitalism in Europe, which was a pioneering attempt to
relate culture from the perspective of religion to economic activ-
ities. Weber also studied religions in China and India, and con-
cluded that, unlike Protestantism, these religions were not condu-
cive to capitalism. This work later inspired a program of research
on the Protestant work ethic in I/O psychology (e.g., Merrens &
Garrett, 1975) and cross-cultural psychology (e.g., Sanchez-Burks,
2002). Other significant events during this time period were the
1919 founding of the International Association of Applied Psy-
chology (IAAP), which, to date, has been an international meeting
ground for organizational scholars worldwide, as well as the es-
tablishment of American Psychological Association’s (APA’s)
Committee on International Relations in Psychology in 1944.
In sum, I/O psychology in the very early years did not pay much
attention to the influence of culture. Despite Wundt’s Volkerpsy-
chologie and Weber’s comparative analysis of different economic
systems, JAP only featured sporadic papers on ethnic differences
(e.g., Garth, Serafini, & Dutton, 1925; Sánchez, 1934), and these
papers were largely descriptive in nature.
Middle 20th Century (1950-1979)
During this period, several significant theoretical, empirical, and
institutional developments occurred in the field of CCIO/OB and
cross-cultural psychology more generally. Against the backdrop of
mainstream psychology’s focus on universal laws of human be-
havior, a critical mass of scholars began to demonstrate wide
variability in psychological processes across cultural groups. The
1950s and 1960s witnessed many seminal studies on culture and
personality (B. B. Whiting & J. W. Whiting, 1975; J. W. Whiting
& Child, 1953), perception (Segall, Campbell, & Herskovits,
1966), motivation (McClelland, 1961), cognition (Witkin & Berry,
1975), and mental abilities (Cronbach & Drenth, 1972) that later
became the bedrock of much of modern day CCIO/OB. The fact
that even “basic” psychological processes were not universal (e.g.,
visual illusions; Segall et al., 1966) was a wakeup call and opened
up the field to the possibility that organizational phenomena might
also be subject to wide cultural variability. This period also wit-
nessed the explication of individualism-collectivism as an impor-
tant dimension of culture (Triandis, 1972), and the notion that
cultural differences can arise as adaptations to ecology (e.g., the
eco-cultural approach; Berry, 1975; Triandis, 1972). During this
time period, we also witnessed significant institutional develop-
ments, including the formation of the International Association for
Cross-Cultural Psychology and the launching of major journals
1
Our review included cross-cultural comparisons (comparison of two or
more cultural groups across nations), intercultural interactions (examina-
tion of two or more groups from different nations who interact with each
other or interactions of ethnically diverse individuals within the same
nation), within-country comparisons across groups (e.g., Asian Americans
vs. Caucasians in the United States), and tests of the generalizability of a
theory typically developed in the U.S. within another nation (e.g., exami-
nation of a Western theory in another country). We also separately coded
the number of articles that were conducted outside of the United States that
were not explicitly focused on culture per se to illustrate how culturally
diverse the populations are in JAP across the last 100 years.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
515
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
(e.g., International Journal of Psychology, Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology).
There were also a number of important scholarly works pub-
lished during this period within CCIO/OB. Harbison and Myers’s
(1959) Management in the Industrial World presented a large-
scale study of comparative management, and Haire, Ghiselli, and
Porter’s (1966) Managerial Thinking compared managers across
14 countries (see also Stouffer et al., 1949). There were also
several reviews of cross-cultural research in IO/OB during this
time period (Barrett & Bass, 1970; Boddewyn & Nath, 1970;
Roberts, 1970).
In a chapter in the Handbook of I/O Psychology, Barrett and
Bass (1970) nevertheless lamented that culture was largely ignored
in mainstream organizational psychology, and argued that
most research in industrial and organizational psychology is done
within one cultural context. This context puts constraints upon both
our theories and our practical solutions to the organizational problems.
Since we are seldom faced with a range and variation of our variables
which adequately reflect the possibilities of human behavior, we tend
to take a limited view of the field. (p. 1675)
These observations on the state of the science of CCIO/OB were
largely borne out in our review of articles published in JAP during
this period. There were few papers in JAP during this period that
focused on culture (see Figure 1), and the majority of papers
appearing in this period did not advance any theory on expected
cultural differences or similarities. Some articles focused on ex-
amining reliability of established measures in different countries
(e.g., achievement motivation: Gough & Hall, 1964; personnel
inventories: Raubenheimer, 1970; and leadership: Tscheulin,
1973). Others focused on testing whether I/O theories replicated in
other countries, though the discussion of culture was largely post
hoc. For example, research tested the generalizability of Hertz-
berg’s two-factor theory (Hines, 1973), Fiedler’s theory of least
preferred coworker (Bennett, 1977), and Vroom’s expectancy the-
ory of motivation (Matsui & Terai, 1975). Several JAP articles
during this period did begin to investigate differences in manage-
rial beliefs and values across countries. Notably, Hofstede (1976)
published a paper in JAP on values of managers from 40 nation-
alities, which were organized into five clusters (Nordic, Germanic,
Anglo, Latin, and Asian), and Lonner and Adams (1972) described
vocational interests across nine nations. Others explicitly stated a
need to recruit cross-cultural samples to expand the focus of I/O
psychological research. For example, Zurcher (1968) discussed
particularism as an important value in Mexico, and Triandis and
Vassiliou (1972) discussed the role of group orientation (later to be
called “collectivism”) as a predictor of selection decisions in the
United States and Greece.
Much work during this period focused on cross-cultural com-
parisons (e.g., comparisons of two or more cultural groups across
nations; see Figure 2), though there was a line of research on
cross-cultural adaptation, which focused on the development of
specific training interventions to assist employees working in Iran,
Thailand, Central America, and Greece (Fiedler, Mitchell, & Tri-
andis, 1971; Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). Culture was largely
equated with country, and the focus was on main effects of culture
(e.g., Shapira & Bass, 1975; Slocum & Strawser, 1972; Whitehall,
1964; Zurcher, 1968). Very few studies discussed translation pro-
cedures or performed tests of equivalence for their measures.
There was a dearth of theorizing on what explains cultural varia-
tion, though one paper focused on the role of economic develop-
ment in understanding cultural differences in conflict behavior
(e.g., Porat, 1970), and most studies were of a correlational nature
(see Figures 3a to d). In all, research on CCIO/OB began to
increase in Journal of Applied Psychology (JAP) during this pe-
riod, but with few exceptions, research on culture was still largely
theoretical, with little attention to methodological issues that need
to be accounted for in doing cross-cultural research.
Later 20th Century: 1980s-2000
A number of significant changes in the world influenced the
growing awareness of cross-cultural differences in the final de-
cades of the 20th century: the invention of the World Wide Web
(1989), the spread of home computers, the fall of the Berlin Wall
(1989), the opening of relations between China and the West, and
the accelerating process of globalization. These technological and
geopolitical changes resulted in individuals and organizations hav-
ing much more exposure and interaction with people from differ-
ent cultures. Globalization also accelerated worldwide industrial
competition during this period, which attracted researchers’ atten-
tion to studying cultural similarities and differences in quality
circles and teamwork (Erez & Earley, 1993).
During this period, a number of scholarly works on culture were
published, which significantly affected the development of CCIO/
OB. Hofstede’s (1980) seminal book, Culture’s Consequences, of-
fered a typology of cultural values, including individualism-
collectivism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity-
femininity, which enabled comparisons of values among cultures (see
also later work on Confucian Dynamism by Hofstede & Bond, 1988).
Schwartz (1992) published his circumplex of cultural values, which
offered a theoretical taxonomy for understanding value compatibili-
ties and value conflicts. During this era, reviews of culture and
organizations were published in the Handbook of Cross-Cultural
Psychology (Tannenbaum, 1980) and in JAP (Bhagat & McQuaid,
1982). Notably, an entire volume on methods in cross-cultural re-
search appeared in the Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
which provided systematic advice on translations, experiments, sur-
veys, and ethics, among other topics. Van de Vijver and Leung (1997)
provided detailed suggestions on how to establish equivalence of
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
50
1920-1950 1950-1980 1980-2000 2000-present
Focus on culture
No focus on culture, but non-
USA sample
Figure 1. Growth of cross-cultural research over time.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
516
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
measurement and deal with response bias, and discussions of levels of
analysis in cross-cultural research began to appear (Leung, 1989). An
entire volume on cross-cultural I/O psychology, edited by Triandis,
Dunnette, and Hough, was published as Volume 4 of the 1994
Handbook of Industrial Organizational Psychology, and a special
volume, New Perspectives on International Industrial/Organizational
Psychology (Earley & Erez, 1997), appeared as part of the Society for
Industrial and Organizational Psychology book series. The APA also
established Division 52 (International Psychology) during this time
period.
Another important theoretical development in CCIO during this
time period was the publication of Erez and Earley’s (1993)
seminal book entitled Culture, Self-Identity, and Work. Drawing
on other work on culture and self (Markus & Kitayama, 1991;
Triandis, 1989), the authors proposed that culture shapes a per-
son’s self through the process of socialization, and this culture-
based self serves as a basis for evaluating the implication of
different organizational practices for a person’s self-worth and
well-being. It had broad-ranging implications for understanding
why certain organizational practices (e.g., motivational practices,
human resources [HR] practices and work design) would be more
or less motivating depending on the cultural context. This notion of
culture fit was also integrated into a theory of HR practices and
individual motivation during this period (Aycan et al., 2000).
Amid this increasing activity, the volume of papers involving
culture was still relatively low in JAP (see Figure 1). Across all of
these articles, there were several notable trends. First, researchers
began to sample a broader array of geographical regions, though
the authors of these studies were largely from the United States
and other Western countries. Research during this period, much
like the previous period, was largely focused on cross-cultural
comparisons (42%) and testing the generalizability of theories
across countries (38%; see Figure 2). Most of these studies con-
cluded that although some of these theories could be generalized to
other cultures, many needed important modifications. For exam-
ple, an examination of Meyer and Allen’s (1991) three-component
model of organizational commitment did not fully replicate in
South Korea (Ko, Price, & Mueller, 1997). Cross-cultural differ-
ences were also evident in the meaning of organization citizenship
behavior (OCB), with employees from Hong Kong and Japan
regarding some categories of OCB as an expected part of the job,
unlike participants from the United States and Australia, who
considered OCB to be independent of the job requirements (Lam,
Hui, & Law, 1999). Likewise, a test of the goal-setting theory of
motivation in the United States and in Israel demonstrated that
Americans reached similar levels of performance under participa-
tive and assigned goals, yet Israelis performed significantly lower
under assigned than participative goals. The authors argued this
reflected Israelis’ low level of power distance (Erez & Earley,
1987). In the field of leadership, the two-factor structure of lead-
ership behavior (e.g., consideration and initiating structure), which
had repeatedly been found in Euro-American samples, was not
supported in Iran (Ayman & Chemers, 1983). In the field of
conflict, Tinsley (1998) expanded the focus of interest models of
disputing to include status and regulation models, which were
particularly relevant in Japan and Germany, respectively.
Very few studies measured aspects of culture (Figure 3a), and of
all of the CCI/OOB papers published in JAP in the 1980s and
1990s, only three tested for measurement equivalence (e.g., Ghor-
pade, Hattrup, & Lackritz, 1999; te Nijenhuis & van der Flier,
31%
69%
1920 - 1950
71%
10%
19%
1950 - 1980
Cross-cultural
Intercultural
Both cross-cultural
and intercultural
Within-country
comparison
across groups
Wthi
i
n-country
Ge
neralizability of a
theory
42%
8%
12%
38%
1980 - 2000
50%
27%
5%
3%
15%
2000 - present
Figure 2. Percentage of different types of culture studies. Cross-cultural comparisons involved a comparison
of two or more cultural groups across nations; intercultural studies involved an examination of two or more
groups from different nations who interacted with each other or interactions of ethnically diverse individuals
within the same nation; within-country comparisons across groups were investigations of group differences with
a particular nation (e.g., Asian Americans versus Caucasians in the United States); within-country test of the
generalizability of a Western theory in another country.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
517
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
1997; Zeidner, 1987). Most JAP studies in this period focused on
culture as a main effect similar to the previous period (Figure 3b),
with a few exceptions. Gelfand and Realo (1999) showed that
accountability (as a norm enforcement mechanism) can produce
opposite effects in individualistic and collectivistic samples, and
Erez and Earley (1987) demonstrated that culture moderated the
effect of goal setting on performance. The importance of studying
culture in context was also beginning to feature in other psychol-
ogy journals as well (Aycan, Kanungo, & Sinha, 1999; Earley,
1993).
Other notable trends in JAP are shown in the figures. Supple-
mental Figure 1 of the online supplemental materials shows that
the topics of interest started to diversify considerably compared
with the previous period. During this time period, there were also
a few studies that included concepts from other cultures that were
not discussed in the mainstream IO and OB literature. For exam-
ple, a comparison of perceived fairness in selection procedures
revealed that graphology was more positively perceived in France
than in the United States (Steiner & Gilliland, 1996). During this
time period, the methods and samples used to study CCIO/OB also
began to diversify (see Figures 3b and 3d).
Emerging Sophistication: The 21st Century
(2000 to Present)
As with other periods, several notable societal and scientific
events have affected the evolution of CCIO/OB over the last 15
years. The aging workforce, especially in Western developed
economies, has dramatically increased the demand to attract and
retain talent from diverse cultural backgrounds (Burke & Ng,
2006). However, human crises, especially the terrorist attacks of
September 11, 2001, also brought forth a hesitancy to embrace
cultural diversity (Burke & Ng, 2006). After this traumatic event,
human resource management (HRM) practices in organizations in
the United States were found to be more conservative and discrim-
inatory, which has likely challenged efforts to understand and
manage cultural diversity in organizations (e.g., Morgan, 2004).
Another important global trend in this era is the increasing
emphasis on cultural diversity in universities, and particularly in
MBA curricula, in which the introduction of MOOCs (massive
open online courses) provided incredible momentum to the inter-
nationalization of higher education in the world. Additionally,
university ranking systems now include “internationalization” as
an important criterion to evaluate universities in the world
(Stromquist, 2007). In response to this trend, several special issues
are published on how to teach cross-cultural management effec-
tively in business schools (e.g., Eisenberg, Härtel, & Stahl, 2013).
The quantum increase in the use of the Internet and social media
has not only connected culturally diverse populations but also
procured “big data,” which are available to be analyzed by cross-
cultural researchers (see Bail, 2014; Castells, 2004). Furthermore,
global, longitudinal, and open-access data sets began to capture
societal values and practices, including the International Social
Survey Programme (http://www.issp.org/index.php) and the
World Values Survey (http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs
.jsp). Within this context, the debate on whether or not cultures
converge or diverge in the face of globalization continued with
0
20
40
60
80
100
1920 -
1950
1950 -
1980
1980 -
2000
2000 -
present
b. Design Percentage Breakdown of
Studies about Culture
Experiment
Correlational Lab
Study
Meta-analysis
Field Experiment
Correlational Field
Study
Theory or Review
paper
0
20
40
60
80
100
1920 -
1950
1950 -
1980
1980 -
2000
2000 -
present
d. Percentage Breakdown of Culture
Studies’ Populations
Employee
Student
Other
0
10
20
30
40
50
1920 - 19501950 - 19801980 - 2000 2000 -
present
a. Percentage of Cultural Studies
that Measure Culture
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
1920 -
1950
1950 -
1980
1980 -
2000
2000 -
present
c. Percent of Main Effect vs.
Moderated Effect Studies
Main Effect
Moderation
Figure 3. Characteristics of articles about culture.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
518
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
great force. Two key publications stirred the debate: One argued
that globalization is making the world “flat” (Friedman, 2005), and
the other argued against globalization producing homogeneity
(Klein, 2002). Project GLOBE, which showed that cultural differ-
ences in values and practices continue to persist, provided support
for the latter perspective (Chhokar, Brodbeck, & House, 2008;
House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004).
Along with these societal developments, there have been nu-
merous theoretical advancements in the field. Most notably, there
is a growing recognition that “country” may not be the most
appropriate unit of analysis for cross-cultural research (Fischer,
2009; Minkov & Hofstede, 2014; Taras, Steel, & Kirkman, 2016),
and that cross-cultural variability can be captured at the state,
ethnic/racial, religious, or socioeconomic status (SES) levels
within countries (e.g., Dheer, Lenartowicz, Peterson, & Petrescu,
2014; Greenfield, 2014; Harrington & Gelfand, 2014; Markus &
Conner, 2014; Yamawaki, 2012). Relatedly, there is a shift from
static to dynamic views of culture. Developments in the cognitive
sciences have stimulated the view of culture as a loose network of
multiple, and sometimes conflicting, knowledge structures that can
be activated (or suppressed) depending on the demands of the
situation. This view is contrasted with a conception of culture as a
set of stable structures (e.g., value orientations) and has produced
research on cultural frame shifting, wherein individuals can dy-
namically integrate and dissociate from elements of their culture
(e.g., Benet-Martínez, Leu, Lee, & Morris, 2002; Hong, Morris,
Chiu, & Benet-Martínez, 2000; Shore, 1996). The constructs of
cultural intelligence and global identity were also advanced to help
explain adaptation to the global work context (Arnett, 2000; Erez
et al., 2013; Lisak & Erez, 2015; Rockstuhl, Seiler, Ang, Van
Dyne, & Annen, 2011).
This era has also witnessed extensions of the value frameworks
predominantly used in CCIO/OB and cross-cultural psychology
more generally (see the special issue of the Journal of Interna-
tional Business Studies edited by Devinney, Kirkman, Caprar, &
Caligiuri, (2015); see also Tung & Verbeke, 2010). Scholars began
to broaden the scope of cultural difference variables to include
beliefs (e.g., social axioms: Leung & Bond, 2004) and norms (e.g.,
tightness-looseness: Chiu, Gelfand, Yamagishi, Shteynberg, &
Wan, 2010; Gelfand, Nishii, & Raver, 2006; Gelfand et al., 2011).
There have also been calls for deeper and fine-grained understand-
ing of the construct of individualism and collectivism (e.g., Yam-
agishi, 2011). Interestingly, during this period, there has been a
return to the 1960s and 1970s focus of understanding the ecolog-
ical and historical bases of culture (Berry, 1975; Triandis, 1972),
such as climate and subsistence systems (e.g., Van de Vliert,
2013), societal threat (Gelfand et al., 2011), and even genetic
characteristics of populations (e.g., Minkov, Blagoev, & Bond,
2015).
Research has moved beyond main effects to examine complex
Culture
Context interactions (Aycan, 2005; Gelfand et al., 2013;
Nouri et al., 2013, 2015), and research has increasingly examined
intercultural interactions. Examples include research on multicul-
tural colocated and virtual teams (Erez et al., 2013; Hülsheger,
Anderson, & Salgado, 2009; Stahl, Maznevski, Voigt, & Jonsen,
2010), expatriate or global managers (Shaffer, Harrison, Gregersen,
Black, & Ferzandi, 2006; Stahl & Caligiuri, 2005), and computer-
mediated cross-cultural interactions (Vignovic & Thompson, 2010).
Recent methodological developments have also allowed cross-
cultural research to broaden its scope. Online surveys using panel
data (e.g., MTurk, Qualtrics) have made data collection from many
countries easy, though these platforms are not without limitations
(Mason & Suri, 2012). Researchers are more frequently testing
measurement invariance or equivalence (Chen, 2008), and are
increasingly using numerous data analytical strategies (e.g., mod-
erated meditational analyses, multilevel models, nested hierarchi-
cal clustering to reveal cultural clusters; M. W.-L. Cheung, Leung,
& Au, 2006; Ronen & Shenkar, 2013). Research has employed
creative methodologies, such as Google Ngram viewer, to trace
changes in cultural values (Greenfield, 2013).
This growing theoretical and methodological sophistication of
the study of culture can clearly be seen in the evolution of this
work in JAP over the last 15 years. There has been a rapid increase
in the number of papers that focus on culture in the journal, and an
exponential increase in the number of studies that are conducted on
non-U.S. samples that do not have a focus on culture (see Figure
1). The diversity of topics has increased, with topics such as
work–family conflict, counterproductive work behavior (sexual
harassment, abusive supervision, mistreatment), psychological
contracts, stress, and turnover adapting a cross-cultural lens, and
the first work on culture and emotions (e.g., shame and guilt)
appeared (supplemental Figure 1). Studies of intercultural interac-
tions have also dramatically increased (see Figure 2). Many papers
have begun to measure culture (Figure 3a) and moved beyond
main effects to explore moderators of cultural differences (Figure
3c). The diversity of methods and samples has also increased (see
Figures 3b and 3d).
As with the larger field, JAP has featured increasing theoretical
complexity in the study of culture. Chao and Moon (2005) pro-
posed a new model of culture using the metaphor of a mosaic to
identify demographic, geographical, and associative features to
describe the unique cultural identity of individuals, and Gelfand et
al. (2006) advanced a multilevel theory of cultural tightness-
looseness in organizations. Others advanced the classic work by
Hofstede (1980) to show when values such as individualism-
collectivism will exert stronger or weaker effects. For example, in
their seminal meta-analysis, Taras, Kirkman, and Steel (2010)
found that compared with personality and demographics, cultural
values had stronger associations with organizational attitudes and
weaker associations with performance, absenteeism, and turnover.
They also showed how norms (i.e., tightness) strengthened the
effect of value on organizational outcomes.
Another exciting trend in CCIO/OB has been a focus on under-
standing work behavior through emic (i.e., culture-specific) con-
structs. For example, guanxi (the Chinese concept of the social
relationships that facilitate networking; Huang & Wang, 2011)is
mentioned in 36 articles in the Academy of Management Journal
(AMJ) and in 16 articles in JAP. The Japanese constructs of giri
(the extent to which obligations to roles and duties are fulfilled)
and taimen (the extent to which face is maintained) were also used
to understand cultural differences in conflict (Gelfand et al., 2001).
New dimensions have been added to existing constructs. For
example, Ramesh and Gelfand (2010) expanded the job embed-
dedness model to include family embeddedness—a new construct
salient in the Indian cultural context—to find that it explained
variance in both India and the United States. As they noted, the
study “draws attention to the fact that cross-cultural expansion of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
519
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
theory can illuminate factors that are important in all cultures but
might get limited attention” (p. 818; see also Rockstuhl, Dulebohn,
Ang, & Shore, 2012; Wong, Tjosvold, & Yu, 2005).
During this period, articles in JAP have increasingly moved
beyond comparisons across countries to explore complex dynam-
ics in intercultural contexts (e.g., expatriate adjustment, multicul-
tural teams, negotiation). For example, Stahl and Caligiuri (2005)
studied the adaptation processes of Germans in Japan and the
United States, and found that problem-focused rather than emo-
tional coping strategies were more effective, particularly in high
power distance contexts. Other articles compared the dynamics in
multicultural and monocultural dyads and teams (Adair, Okumura,
& Brett, 2001; Liu, Chua, & Stahl, 2010), and the types of leaders
that are best suited for multicultural teams. For example, Lisak and
Erez (2015) found that emergent leaders in multicultural virtual
teams scored higher than other team members on their glocal
identity (high global, high local identity; see also Greer, Homan,
De Hoogh, and Den Hartog [2012], for an exploration of effective
leaders of ethnically diverse teams).
Articles published in JAP during this time period have tested the
generality of a wide range of classic IO/OB theories. These have
included Karasek’s (1979) decision latitude model of work
(Schaubroeck, Lam, & Xie, 2000), Graen and Uhl-Bien’s (1995)
leader-member exchange theory (Rockstuhl et al., 2012), Rous-
seau’s (2000) psychological contract theory (Hui, Lee, & Rous-
seau, 2004), and Fitzgerald, Hulin, and Drasgow’s (1995) theory
of sexual harassment (Wasti, Bergman, Glomb, & Drasgow,
2000). JAP also published several articles on the validity of scales
used in different countries, such as personality inventories and
measures of organizational citizenship, job satisfaction, resistance
to change, and multisource feedback in performance evaluation,
among others (e.g., Liu et al., 2010; Oreg et al., 2008). Several JAP
articles also investigated complex interactions between culture and
context. For example, Gelfand et al. (2013) used moderated me-
diation to show that descriptive norms can explain culture by
context interactions in team and solo negotiations in the United
States and Taiwan (see also Fisher, 2014, for a multilevel exam-
ination of role overload, empowering organizational climate, and
culture).
Finally, we see increasing methodological sophistication of
cross-cultural research published in JAP in this era. Research is
increasingly moving beyond two country comparisons to explore,
in some cases, over 20 countries (i.e., Atwater, Wang, Smither, &
Fleenor, 2009; Peretz & Fried, 2012; Rockstuhl et al., 2012;
Sturman, Shao, & Katz, 2012). Though still low in frequency,
there have been several studies that have adopted a multiple
method approach, which is particularly important in cross-cultural
research (Gelfand, Raver, & Holcombe Ehrhart, 2002). Cultural
differences are increasingly measured rather than assumed, and
attention to translations and measurement equivalence, although
still somewhat low, is gradually increasing.
Summary: 100 Years of Cross-Cultural
Research in JAP
In this hundred-year journey, we witness the evolution of
cross-cultural IO/OB research. Across each historical period,
from the early years to the mid- to late 20th century, to the 21st
century and beyond, we can see how societal and intellectual
events dramatically affected the course of CCIO/OB science.
The motivations for doing cross-cultural research are certainly
diverse, be they testing the generalizability of Western theories;
broadening existing theories; developing new measures and
theories of cultural variation; comparing main, moderating,
and/or multilevel effects across many areas of CCIO/OB; or
examining the nature of intercultural interactions and bicultur-
alism. However, the common denominator across all of these
efforts has been recognizing the existence of cultural diversity
and the desire to understand people’s values, norms, and be-
haviors across cultures. As the world is becoming increasingly
more global, understanding cross-cultural similarities and dif-
ferences will enable us to harness the cultural diversity across
dispersed geographical zones and enhance people’s quality of
life around the globe.
In many ways, the field has increased in its scope, diversity,
and theoretical and methodological sophistication. Collectively,
JAP published 102 papers from 1917 to 2014 that explicitly
considered cultural variation, and an additional 50 papers ap-
peared that included populations beyond the United States that
did not have culture as their focus. Figures 1 through 3 illustrate
that the diversity of samples, topics, and methods in CCIO/OB
are increasing at dramatic rates. These figures also illustrate
that although research published in JAP has generally not
assessed aspects of culture (e.g., values, beliefs, norms) that
potentially explain differences in work behavior, the rate of
their assessment is steadily increasing, as is the assessment of
measurement equivalence. Research is moving beyond main
effects to examine culture as a moderator, and the presence of
multilevel models is increasing in the field in general, and JAP
in particular.
Yet amid the progress noted throughout this discussion it is
important to emphasize that the attention to culture in the
journal is actually remarkably low: Of the 9419 papers pub-
lished in JAP between 1917 and 2014, the proportion of articles
that explicitly focus on culture is only 1% (including nonwest-
ern samples without a specific focus on culture in the analysis
brings the total to 1.6%). Moreover, the scope of CCIO/OB is
still very limited. Research on CCIO/OB sampled from a nar-
row range of countries, most notably Europe (34%), North
America (27%), Asia (24%), Africa (3%), Australia (3%), and
South and Central America (9%). As well, the articles reviewed
in JAP showed a high level of Western dominance: 66% of first
authors were American. This is similar to Tsui, Nifadkar, and
Ou’s (2007) analysis of cross-cultural organizational science in
which they found that 68% of the studies’ first authors were
from the U.S. and remarkably, 100% of the 69 unique first
authors were from countries characterized as having “high
human development” (see Kirkman & Law, 2005, for a review
of trends in international management research in AMJ). This
dominance, though largely unintentional, restricts the potential
of a truly global organizational science in terms of the questions
asked, the constructs developed, and the conclusions reached.
Overall, the immense potential for globalization in the field has
yet to be tapped even despite societal and intellectual trends and
global issues that warrant understanding cultural variation and
universality.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
520
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
The Next 100 Years of Cross-Cultural I/O
OB Research
In this final section, we take the opportunity to highlight our
vision for the next 100 years of culture research in IO/OB in
general, and in JAP in particular. As we discuss at length in the
following sections, the field should build on the momentum oc-
curring on numerous theoretical, methodological, and empirical
fronts. In this next 100 years, we need to (a) broaden the questions
we ask in CCIO/OB, (b) give more attention to the conceptualiza-
tion and operationalization of culture, (c) make dynamic perspec-
tives of culture more of the norm than the exception, (d) have an
increased focus on intercultural interactions and the global context
of work, (e) take the meaning of constructs across cultures more
seriously, and (f) increase the methodological and disciplinary
perspectives devoted to the study of culture in IO/OB. Each is
discussed in turn.
Broadening the Questions We Ask in CCIO/OB
Philosophers of science have long argued that science is
value-laden (Kuhn, 1962; Lefkowitz, 2003). As Sampson
(1978) specifically noted, “modern science emerged within a
particular sociohistorical context [in which] the values of lib-
eralism, individualism, capitalism, and male dominance [are]
primary” (p. 1334). Importantly, these values profoundly affect
the questions we find worthy of study as well as those that we
do not (Gelfand, Leslie, & Fehr, 2008).
CCIO/OB research is no exception; it has largely been pio-
neered in the United States and the West, and is laden with
culture-specific values and sociopolitical realities that risk be-
ing exported to other cultures. For example, Gelfand et al.
(2008) noted that theories and research questions in CCIO/OB
largely reflect a cultural model of the independent self, empha-
sizing individual differences, freedom of choice, and the pursuit
of happiness and personal satisfaction, which is made possible
in countries in which there is affluence, industrialization, and
relative social tranquility (Inglehart, 2000). Yet in contrast,
millions of individuals around the globe face daily realities of
poverty, conflict, terrorism, and corruption—where basic needs
and safety concerns loom large—necessitating that different
research questions be asked that are not only vital to individuals
but to societal development. As Gelfand et al. noted, in these
contexts,
the science of job security and unemployment might take precedence
over the science of job satisfaction and commitment; the criteria for
selection systems might focus less on objectivity and validity, and
more on the legitimization of subjectivity and nepotism...thefocus
of training...might be on basic skills such as literacy. (p. 29)
Other Western values permeate our theories and questions asked
in CCIO/OB. For example, Western cultures prioritize bound-
aries (e.g., between work and family, religion and state), yet
such boundaries do not necessarily apply in other cultures. The
influence of religion in the workplace, for example, is rarely
examined in CCIO/OB, yet is likely important in many coun-
tries in the world. In all, we argue that in the next 100 years of
research in CCIO/OB, we need to be mindful that the theories
we develop and questions we ask may be laden with Western
concerns, and we must strive to ask new questions that reflect
other societal values, assumptions, and sociopolitical realities.
Operationalization of Culture Through Measurement
and Attention to Levels of Analysis
The CCIO/OB scholarly community needs to engage in deep,
critical, and multidisciplinary understanding of what culture is
and how it should be best captured. We believe that CCIO/OB
should embrace conceptual diversity when it comes to under-
standing and modeling culture, yet also must be precise regard-
ing the level of theory and measurement being advanced. Gel-
fand et al. (2008) describe various forms of culture that could be
of interest in CCIO/OB, including those at the individual level
(personal values, subjective cultural press), the unit level (ad-
ditive culture, or averages of individual values vs. cultural
descriptive norms, or averages of perceived descriptive norms),
as well as those that reflect dispersion (variance in values or
perceived descriptive norms), all of which might have different
influences on I/O OB phenomena.
Indeed, the recent special issue of the Journal of Interna-
tional Business Studies entitled “What is Culture and How Do
We Measure It?” (Devinney, Kirkman, Caprar, & Caligiuri,
2015) provides additional frameworks for the measurement of
culture, cultural diversity, and cultural distance. For example,
drawing on topology and matrix algebra, Venaik and Midgley
(2015) proposed a methodology called “archetypal analysis” as
an alternative to a value-based approach to measurement of
culture. Archetypes are perfect theoretical representations of
configurations of values shared by a group. The transnational
archetypes identified by the authors cut across different coun-
tries and represent the “etic,” whereas the subnational arche-
types (i.e., unique configurations of value) represent the “emic”
aspect of culture. Hence, this methodology offers a novel way
of reconciling the etic– emic tension. Likewise, building on
methodologies from economics, political science, and ethnog-
raphy, Luiz (2015) proposed an alternative methodology to
assess intranational diversity and cultural distance, which he
referred to as “ethno-linguistic fractionalization.” Future re-
search can use this index to answer such questions as whether
or not expatriates or multinational corporations perform better
in heterogeneous, rather than homogenous, cultures (see also
Mohr & Ghaziani [2014], for advances in sociology to improve
clarity in the conceptualization and operationalization of cul-
ture).
In the next 100 years, the literature also needs to move
beyond studying culture at the national level. National bound-
aries, some of which are admittedly arbitrary, may not always
offer the most appropriate unit of analysis to study culture (see
Fischer, 2009; Taras et al., 2016). As noted, recent research has
discovered cultural differences (i.e., in values and the strength
of norms) across the lines of socioeconomic class, profession,
religion, ethnic groups, age cohorts, and regions within coun-
tries (e.g., Harrington & Gelfand, 2014; Luiz, 2015; Markus &
Conner, 2014; Ronen & Shenkar, 2013; Taras et al., 2010). We
need to develop more complex models that simultaneously
examine how multiple levels of culture—such as global, na-
tional, regional, state, community, industry, organizational, and
team levels—affect behavior in organizations. We also need to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
521
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
understand how national culture dynamically interacts with
SES, gender, and age, among other demographic and personal-
ity attributes, to affect work behavior. More generally, with the
advent of multilevel modeling techniques, it is possible to
examine many different models of cultural influence, ranging
from single-level models to cross-level direct and cross-level
moderated models of culture, as well as to model the simulta-
neous influence of different sources of culture on units, teams,
and individuals (e.g., mixed-determinant models; Kozlowski &
Klein, 2000; see also Gelfand et al., 2008).
Dynamic Perspectives of Culture Should Become the
Norm, Not the Exception
Research has clearly shown that culture can be as important
of a predictor of organizational phenomena as other variables
such as demographics and personality traits (Taras et al., 2010).
We believe the time has now come to devote more attention to
the questions of under what conditions and for which type of
criteria do cross-cultural differences matter the most? Taras et
al. (2010), for example, showed that cultural differences may be
moderated by the type of organizational outcome under inves-
tigation, and that the predictive power of cultural values can be
stronger for certain populations, including employed popula-
tions versus students, older rather than younger respondents,
men compared with women, more educated populations rather
than less educated ones, and in tight rather than loose societies.
Other contingencies under which cultures’ consequences
vary need to be examined in the next 100 years. Aycan (2005),
for example, argued that the impact of culture on HRM would
be less evident in large, publically traded organizations, or
those operating in industries that use sophisticated technologies
(e.g., IT sector), compared with small to medium sized family-
owned organizations or those operating in industries that use
semiskilled employees (e.g., manufacturing). Similarly, Gib-
son, Maznevski, and Kirkman (2009) proposed that culture’s
impact will be more pronounced on individuals who are high in
conformity, conscientiousness, openness, and adaptability, as
well as among individuals who had have high identification
with their own culture and low exposure to other cultures. The
authors also identified contingencies under which culture’s
impact was stronger on groups, such as when group identifica-
tion, cohesion, and homogeneity is high, and group polarization
is low.
Theory is needed to explain precisely why and how situa-
tional contingencies intensify or exacerbate cross-cultural dif-
ferences. For example, the situated dynamics (SD) framework
of culture by Leung and Morris (2015) expands the value-based
view of culture to include schemas and norms, which are
situation sensitive. The SD framework postulates that values
play a stronger role in situations involving social adaptation
signals or cues to moral/ethical decisions, whereas behavioral
schemas or norms play a greater role in situations involving
interpretive or behavioral tasks. The framework reconciles ten-
sions between cultural stability and culture change by positing
that some aspects of culture (e.g., values) are relatively consis-
tent across time, whereas others (e.g., schemas and norms) are
in flux with situational demands. Similarly, Zellmer-Bruhn and
Gibson (2014) proposed a promising framework highlighting
the role of situational context, and in particular, the concept of
intercultural interaction space to denote the physical, cognitive,
and affective characteristics of the situation in which interac-
tions occur and how these characteristics attenuate or augment
cultural differences. In all, few JAP articles have focused on
complex interactions between culture and context in the last 100
years. Future CCIO/OB research in JAP should go beyond the
question of whether or not culture matters and focus on when
and how it matters (see also Erez, Lee, & Van de Ven, 2015;
Nouri et al., 2015; Zhou & Su, 2010).
From Intracultural Comparisons With Intercultural
Interactions and the Global Work Context
Although our review illustrates increasing complexity with
which culture is theorized and studied, there still exists a dominant
paradigm of studying culture as a static main effect, largely in
correlational field studies. The future of culture research, we
believe, will need to capture cultural dynamics (i.e., change and
interaction) at all levels of analysis, including the dynamics of
cultural frame shifting within individuals, the dynamics of inter-
cultural negotiations and virtual teams, the multitude of factors that
affect the success or failure of cross-cultural mergers and acqui-
sitions, and large-scale cultural changes around the globe that
result from rapid top-down and bottom-up ecological, demo-
graphic, and market forces.
Additionally, the increasingly globalized, networked world ne-
cessitates that we understand how the global context of a culturally
diverse and geographically dispersed workforce changes our the-
ories, research questions, and methodologies. Kraimer, Takeuchi,
and Frese (2014) defined global work context to include
any job-related activities that involve interacting with people from
other countries. Examples include interacting with customers or co-
workers from foreign countries, working in cross-national teams,
having extensive international travel requirements as part of the job,
and living and working in a foreign country for extended periods of
time (whether self- or corporate-initiated). (p. 6)
In the globalized work context, we expect more people to live
and work in more than one culture, be it their home culture and
host culture, or the global cultural context more generally.
This trend opens new and exciting research avenues for
CCIO/OB scholars to address. First, how do we conceptualize and
measure “global culture”? Although research has been published
on the national culture and on subcultures at the organizational and
team levels, there has been a dearth of attention to the meaning
and impact of global culture. Second, how do people negotiate
multiple identities depending on the demands of different con-
texts? Erez and colleagues argue that global work context contrib-
utes to the development of global identities independent of any
national local identity (Erez & Gati, 2004; Erez et al., 2013;
Shokef & Erez, 2008). Based on the recent theoretical advance-
ments explained in previous sections, CCIO/OB research should
investigate whether increasing exposure to the global work context
attenuates cultural differences. For example, Glikson and Erez
(2013) showed that variance in emotion display norms was
smaller in the global context than in the local cultural contexts.
In addition, it will be critical to examine how people manage
cross-cultural interactions in the global work context. Given the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
522
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
fact that cross-cultural interactions tend to be more “frequent,
horizontal, unstructured, temporary, sporadic, and across global
locations” (Zellmer-Bruhn & Gibson, 2014, p. 3), we must
invest in theorizing and researching “cross-cultural interac-
tions” more than we do in “cross-cultural differences.” Within
this spirit, CCIO/OB research should be in closer contact with
the cultural intelligence and diversity management literatures to
develop theories capturing processes and outcomes of cross-
cultural interactions. Finally, how do global teams work effec-
tively? We expect a significant growth in the presence of
multicultural virtual teams, given the fast development of in-
formation communication technologies (ICT; Gibson, Huang,
Kirkman, & Shapiro, 2014). Future research should investigate
how ICTs enable the emergence of shared norms in multicul-
tural teams (see the special issue on culture and collaboration in
Journal of Organizational Behavior: Salas & Gelfand, 2013).
We should also have more research on global leaders who
manage multicultural teams and identify what makes them
successful in the global context (Lisak & Erez, 2015).
Take Meaning of Constructs Across Cultures
More Seriously
An important challenge for future CCIO/OB research is to
explore “universal” constructs (i.e., etics) as well as “culturally
embedded” unique constructs or (i.e., emics). We believe that
discoveries of emic constructs and expanding our existing con-
structs to include emic dimensions will ultimately enable us to
develop a truly global universal science.
Research is indeed increasingly illustrating that we need to
expand upon many of our existing constructs for them to be
relevant beyond the West. For example, the latent construct of
personality for Chinese goes beyond the five-factor model of
McCrae and Costa (1997) to include emic dimensions such as
Reng Qing (adherence to norms of interaction), Ah-Q (external-
ization of blame), Harmony (inner peace and interpersonal har-
mony), and Face (e.g., F. M. Cheung et al., 1996). Similarly,
organizational citizenship behaviors include additional dimensions
such as self-training, protecting and saving company resources,
and keeping the workplace clean among Chinese, and the behav-
iors that constitute the same dimensions as those in the Western
literature vary in meaningful ways (Farh, Zhong, & Organ, 2004).
Relatedly, Lowenstein and Mueller (2016) found significant dif-
ferences across cultures in the meaning of creativity, with a broad
meaning in China, including usefulness and, harmony, and a more
narrow meeting of creativity in the United States (see also Gibson
& Zellmer-Bruhn, 2001, for differences in metaphors for teams
across cultures). Put differently, from a psychometric point of
view, it is critical to ensure that our constructs are not deficient—
that is, missing important dimensions that are relevant in other
cultures. In this spirit, we caution that establishing measurement
equivalence of scales through factor analysis does not guarantee
universality of the constructs being studied.
More generally, there needs to be more institutional support for
publishing indigenous or emic dimensions of constructs in the
field. For example, emic constructs such as guanxi, wasta, giri,
taimen, paternalism, jeitinho, and yuan, among others, need to be
integrated into I/O OB research (see Smith, 2008; Tung & Aycan,
2008). We also need to be cognizant that research with U.S.
samples may represent culture-specific phenomena, that is, that of
American, rather than “universal,” phenomena (e.g., Danziger,
2006).
2
Importantly, studying indigenous or emic phenomena,
wherever they originate, is not incompatible with building a global
science; indeed, it is the path to a truly universal science of
psychology. As Pruitt (2004, p. xii) states, “Characteristics that are
dominant in one culture tend to be recessive in another, and vice
versa.” Moreover, by gaining knowledge on emic constructs, we
can begin to build broader theories across cultures. For example,
though guanxi has specific emic elements that define it, it also has
some commonality with other constructs related to social capital
(see Huang, & Wang, 2011; Qi, 2013; Smith, Huang, Harb, &
Torres, 2012).
We would also argue that the development of such constructs is
not only important for science—indeed, Marsden (1991) argues
that sustainable national and organizational development is more
likely if emic or indigenous constructs are understood and utilized
better:
Indigenous knowledge...maybethebasis for building more
sustainable development strategies, because they begin from where
the people are, rather than from where development experts would
like them to be. It is commonly maintained that these indigenous
knowledge systems, if articulated properly, will provide the bases for
increasing productivity. (p. 31)
Capturing emic realities is therefore critical for ultimately advanc-
ing practice.
Methodological and Disciplinary Diversity Should
be a Priority
As culture is a complex phenomenon, we must strive to have
methodological and epistemological diversity in the field. For
example, we need to complement existing quantitative methods
with those that are more qualitative in nature, the latter of which
received no attention in CCI/OOB research published in JAP
during the last 100 years (see Cole, 2006; Vygotsky & Wollock,
1997; and see Karasz & Singelis, 2009 for a special issue of the
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology on qualitative and mixed
methodologies in cross-cultural research). Likewise, other exper-
imental methodologies, such as priming cultural values (e.g., Co-
hen, Montoya, & Insko, 2006; Oyserman & Lee, 2008), should
complement field and qualitative data. Indeed, we observed that
few studies in JAP utilize multiple methods, which is essential for
triangulation and ruling out rival hypotheses (Gelfand et al., 2002).
More broadly, it will be critical for CCIO/OB scholars to partner
with scholars in other disciplines, including those in computer
science, linguistics, neuroscience, biology, and history, among
others, to increase our theoretical and methodological diversity.
Rapid developments in cognitive neuroscience have stimulated the
emergence of new fields, such as “cultural neuroscience” and
2
In our review of JAP articles, we observed that almost all of the JAP
articles with non-U.S. samples included a sentence of caveat about the
generalizability of findings to other cultural contexts, whereas no such
warning was evident in articles with U.S. samples, which tend to assume
that the phenomenon is generalizable rather than unique to Americans. As
a general practice, we believe that all articles should question whether
findings are generalizable to other contexts and under what conditions they
may or may not.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
523
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
“sociogenetics” (e.g., Minkov et al., 2015; see also Chiao &
Blizinsky, 2010; Mrazek, Chiao, Blizinsky, Lun, & Gelfand,
2013). As well, integrating computer science perspectives into
cross-cultural research is increasingly yielding new insights into
the evolution of cultural differences relevant to I/O and OB (see
Nowak, Gelfand, Borkowski, Cohen, & Hernandez, 2016; Roos,
Gelfand, Nau, & Carr, 2014; Roos, Gelfand, Nau, & Lun, 2015).
It is our hope that JAP will encourage submissions by interdisci-
plinary and international teams featuring state-of-the-art ap-
proaches to allow for a deeper understanding of what culture is and
how it should be best captured.
Conclusion
Cross-cultural research in JAP has evolved significantly over
the last 100 years. Although culture was largely ignored in the
early years and through the mid-20th century, there has been a
great increase in the momentum in the field, particularly in the last
decade, that promises to continue in the next 100 years. Cross-
cultural research is needed more than ever to understand and
leverage similarities and differences in an ever-more increasingly
globalized and interdependent world.
References
Adair, W. L., Okumura, T., & Brett, J. M. (2001). Negotiation behavior
when cultures collide: The United States and Japan. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 86, 371–385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.371
Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. Advances in Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 2, 267–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-
2601(08)60108-2
Antonakis, J. (2011). Predictors of leadership: The usual suspects and the
suspect traits. In A. Bryman, D. Collinson, K. Grint, B. Jackson, & M.
Uhl-Bien (Eds.), Sage handbook of leadership (pp. 269–285). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Araujo, S. F. (2013). Völkerpsychologie. In K. D. Keigh (Ed.), The
encyclopedia of cross-cultural psychology (pp. 1333–1338). Chichester,
UK: Wiley Blackwell. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118339893
.wbeccp560
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469
480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469
Atwater, L., Wang, M., Smither, J. W., & Fleenor, J. W. (2009). Are
cultural characteristics associated with the relationship between self and
others’ ratings of leadership? Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 876
886. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014561
Aycan, Z. (2005). The interplay between cultural and institutional/
structural contingencies in human resource management practices. The
International Journal of Human Resource Management, 16, 1083–1119.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585190500143956
Aycan, Z., Kanungo, R., Mendonca, M., Yu, K., Deller, J., Stahl, G., &
Kurshid, A. (2000). Impact of culture on human resource management
practices: A 10-country comparison. Applied Psychology: An Interna-
tional Review, 49, 192–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00010
Aycan, Z., Kanungo, R. N., & Sinha, J. B. P. (1999). Organizational culture
and human resource management practices: The model of culture fit.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 501–526. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0022022199030004006
Ayman, R., & Chemers, M. M. (1983). Relationship of supervisory be-
havior ratings to work group effectiveness and subordinate satisfaction
among Iranian managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 338–341.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.68.2.338
Bail, C. A. (2014). The cultural environment: Measuring culture with big
data. Theory and Society, 43, 465–482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s11186-014-9216-5
Barrett, G. V., & Bass, B. M. (1970). Comparative surveys of managerial
attitudes and behaviors (No. TR-36). Rochester, NY: Rochester Univer-
sity Management Research Center.
Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating
biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional
versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-
ogy, 33, 492–516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033005005
Bennett, M. (1977). Testing management theories cross-culturally. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 62, 578 –581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
9010.62.5.578
Berry, J. W. (1975). An ecological approach to cross-cultural psychology.
Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haar Grensgebieden, 30,
51– 84.
Bhagat, R. S., & McQuaid, S. J. (1982). Role of subjective culture in
organizations: A review and directions for future research. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 67, 653– 685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010
.67.5.653
Boddewyn, J., & Nath, R. (1970). Comparative management studies: An
assessment. Management International Review, 10, 3–11.
Burke, R. J., & Ng, E. (2006). The changing nature of work and organi-
zations: Implications for human resource management. Human Resource
Management Review, 16, 86–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2006
.03.006
Castells, M. (2004). The network society: A cross-cultural perspective.
Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.
Chao, G. T., & Moon, H. (2005). The cultural mosaic: A metatheory for
understanding the complexity of culture. Journal of Applied Psychology,
90, 1128 –1140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1128
Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks?
The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural re-
search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1005–1018.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013193
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Fan, R. M., Song, W. Z., Zhang, J. X., & Zhang,
J. P. (1996). Development of the Chinese personality assessment inven-
tory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 181–199. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0022022196272003
Cheung, M. W.-L., Leung, K., & Au, K. (2006). Evaluating multilevel
models in cross-cultural research: An illustration with social axioms.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 522–541. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0022022106290476
Chhokar, J. S., Brodbeck, F. C., & House, R. J. (2008). Culture and
leadership across the world: The GLOBE Book of In-Depth Studies of 25
Societies. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Chiao, J. Y., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture-gene coevolution of
individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceed-
ings Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 277, 529 –537. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1650
Chiu, C.-Y., Gelfand, M. J., Yamagishi, T., Shteynberg, G., & Wan, C.
(2010). Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective perceptions in
cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 482–
493. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375562
Cohen, T. R., Montoya, R. M., & Insko, C. A. (2006). Group morality and
intergroup relations: Cross-cultural and experimental evidence. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1559–1572. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0146167206291673
Cole, M. (2006). Culture and cognitive development in phylogenetic,
historical, and ontogenetic perspective. In D. Kuhn, R. S. Seigler, W.
Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child development (pp.
636 683). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Cronbach, L. J., & Drenth, P. J. D. (Eds.). (1972). Mental tests and cultural
adaptation. The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
524
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
Danziger, K. (2006). Universalism and indigenization in the history of
modern psychology. In A. C. Brock (Ed.), Internationalizing the history
of psychology (pp. 208 –225). New York, NY: New York University
Press.
Devinney, T. M., Kirkman, B. L., Caprar, D. V., & Caligiuri, P. (2015).
What is culture and how do we measure it? [Special issue]. Journal of
International Business Studies, 46(9).
Dheer, R., Lenartowicz, T., Peterson, M. F., & Petrescu, M. (2014).
Cultural regions of Canada and United States: Implications for interna-
tional management research. International Journal of Cross Cultural
Management, 14, 343–384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1470595
814543706
Earley, P. C. (1993). East meets west meets Mideast: Further explorations
of collectivistic and individualistic work groups. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 36, 319 –348. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256525
Earley, P. C., & Erez, M. (Eds.). (1997). New perspectives on international
industrial/organizational psychology. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), Frontiers of
industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 160–189). San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
Eisenberg, J., Härtel, C. E., & Stahl, G. K. (2013). From the guest editors:
Cross-cultural management learning and education—Exploring multiple
aims, approaches, and impacts. Academy of Management Learning &
Education, 12, 323–329.
Erez, M., & Earley, P. C. (1987). Comparative analysis of goal-setting
strategies across cultures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 658 665.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.72.4.658
Erez, M., & Earley, P. C. (1993). Culture, self-identity, and work. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:
oso/9780195075809.001.0001
Erez, M., & Gati, E. (2004). A dynamic, multi-level model of culture: From
the micro level of the individual to the macro level of a global culture.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53, 583–598. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00190.x
Erez, M., Lee, C., & Van de Ven, A. H. (2015). Contextualizing creativity
and innovation across cultures. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36,
895– 898.
Erez, M., Lisak, A., Harush, R., Glikson, E., Nouri, R., & Shokef, E.
(2013). Going global: Developing management students’ cultural intel-
ligence and global identity in culturally diverse virtual teams. Academy
of Management Learning & Education, 12, 330–355. http://dx.doi.org/
10.5465/amle.2012.0200
Farh, J. L., Zhong, C. B., & Organ, D. W. (2004). Organizational citizen-
ship behavior in the People’s Republic of China. Organization Science,
15, 241–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1030.0051
Fiedler, F. E., Mitchell, T., & Triandis, H. C. (1971). The culture assimi-
lator: An approach to cross-cultural training. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 55, 95–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0030704
Fischer, R. (2009). Where is culture in cross cultural research? An outline
of a multilevel research process for measuring culture as a shared
meaning system. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management,
9, 25– 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1470595808101154
Fisher, D. M. (2014). A multilevel cross-cultural examination of role
overload and organizational commitment: Investigating the interactive
effects of context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 723–736. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035861
Fitzgerald, L. F., Hulin, C. L., & Drasgow, F. (1995). The antecedents and
consequences of sexual harassment in organizations: An integrated
model. In G. Keita & J. Hurrell, Jr. (Eds.), Job stress in a changing
workforce: Investigating gender, diversity, and family issues (pp. 55–
73). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Friedman, T. L. (2005). The world is flat. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus &
Giroux.
Frost, R. (1920). What industry wants and does not want from the psy-
chologist. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 18–24. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/h0073012
Garth, T. R., Serafini, T. J., & Dutton, D. (1925). The intelligence of full
blood Indians. Journal of Applied Psychology, 9, 382–389. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/h0075158
Gelfand, M. J., Brett, J., Gunia, B. C., Imai, L., Huang, T.-J., & Hsu, B.-F.
(2013). Toward a culture-by-context perspective on negotiation: Nego-
tiating teams in the United States and Taiwan. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 98, 504 –513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031908
Gelfand, M. J., Leslie, L. M., & Fehr, R. (2008). To prosper, organizational
psychology should...adopt a global perspective. Journal of Organi-
zational Behavior, 29, 493–517. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.530
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., Holcombe, K. M., Dyer, N., Ohbuchi, K. I.,
& Fukuno, M. (2001). Cultural influences on cognitive representations
of conflict: Interpretations of conflict episodes in the United States and
Japan. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 1059–1074. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0021-9010.86.6.1059
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., & Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and
importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 91, 1225–1244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1225
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., & Holcombe Ehrhart, K. (2002). Methodolog-
ical issues in cross-cultural organizational research. In S. Rogelberg
(Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology research
methods (pp. 216 –241). New York, NY: Blackwell.
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C.,
. . . Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures:
A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 1100 –1104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/
science.1197754
Gelfand, M. J., & Realo, A. (1999). Individualism-collectivism and ac-
countability in intergroup negotiations. Journal of Applied Psychology,
84, 721–736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.721
Ghorpade, J., Hattrup, K., & Lackritz, J. R. (1999). The use of personality
measures in cross-cultural research: A test of three personality scales
across two countries. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 670 679.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.670
Gibson, C. B., Huang, L., Kirkman, B. L., & Shapiro, D. L. (2014). Where
global and virtual meet: The value of examining the intersection of these
elements in twenty-first century teams. Annual Review of Organiza-
tional Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 217–244. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091240
Gibson, C. B., Maznevski, M. L., & Kirkman, B. L. (2009). When does
culture matter? In R. S. Bhagat & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Cambridge
handbook of culture, organizations, and work (pp. 46 68). Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
CBO9780511581151.004
Gibson, C. B., & Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E. (2001). Metaphors and meaning:
An intercultural analysis of the concept of teamwork. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 46, 274 –303. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2667088
Glikson, E., & Erez, M. (2013). Emotion display norms in virtual teams.
Journal of Personnel Psychology, 12, 22–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/
1866-5888/a000078
Gough, H. G., & Hall, W. B. (1964). Prediction of performance in medical
school from the California Psychological Inventory. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 48, 218 –226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0041092
Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to
leadership: Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of
leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspec-
tive. The Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
1048-9843(95)90036-5
Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The changing psychology of culture from 1800
through 2000. Psychological Science, 24, 1722–1731. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0956797613479387
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
525
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
Greenfield, P. M. (2014). Sociodemographic differences within countries
produce variable cultural values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
45, 37– 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022113513402
Greer, L. L., Homan, A. C., De Hoogh, A. H. B., & Den Hartog, D. N.
(2012). Tainted visions: The effect of visionary leader behaviors and
leader categorization tendencies on the financial performance of ethni-
cally diverse teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 203–213. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025583
Haire, M., Ghiselli, E. E., & Porter, L. W. (1966). Managerial thinking: An
international study. Oxford, UK: Wiley.
Harbison, F., & Myers, C. A. (1959). Management in the industrial world.
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Harrington, J. R., & Gelfand, M. J. (2014). Tightness–looseness across the
50 United States. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
ences of the United States of America, 111, 7990 –7995. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1073/pnas.1317937111
Hines, G. H. (1973). Achievement motivation, occupations, and labor
turnover in New Zealand. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 313–317.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036300
Hofstede, G. (1976). Nationality and espoused values of managers. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 61, 148 –155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
9010.61.2.148
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in
thinking and organizing. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1988). The Confucius connection: From
cultural roots to economic growth. Organizational Dynamics, 16, 5–21.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0090-2616(88)90009-5
Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multi-
cultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cog-
nition. American Psychologist, 55, 709 –720. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0003-066X.55.7.709
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V.
(2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Huang, K., & Wang, K. Y. (2011). How is guanxi related to social capital?
A psychological perspective. Journal of Social Sciences, 7, 120–126.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3844/jssp.2011.120.126
Hui, C., Lee, C., & Rousseau, D. M. (2004). Psychological contract and
organizational citizenship behavior in China: Investigating generaliz-
ability and instrumentality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 311–321.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.311
Hülsheger, U. R., Anderson, N., & Salgado, J. F. (2009). Team-level
predictors of innovation at work: A comprehensive meta-analysis span-
ning three decades of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94,
1128 –1145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015978
Inglehart, R. (2000). Culture and democracy. In L. E. Harrison & S. P.
Huntington (Eds.), Culture matters: How values shape human progress
(pp. 80 –97). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental
strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly,
24, 285–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392498
Karasz, A., & Singelis, T. M. (2009). Qualitative and mixed methods
research in cross-cultural psychology: Introduction to the special issue.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 909–916. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0022022109349172
Katzell, R. A., & Austin, J. T. (1992). From then to now: The development
of industrial-organizational psychology in the United States. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 77, 803– 835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010
.77.6.803
Kirkman, B. L., & Law, K. S. (2005). International management research
in AMJ: Our past, present, and future. Academy of Management Journal,
48,
377–386. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2005.17407902
Klein, N. (2002). Fences and windows: Dispatches from the front lines of
globalization debate. London, UK: Flamingo Press.
Ko, J.-W., Price, J. L., & Mueller, C. W. (1997). Assessment of Meyer and
Allen’s three-component model of organizational commitment in South
Korea. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 961–973. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0021-9010.82.6.961
Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Klein, K. J. (2000). A multilevel approach to theory
and research in organizations. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski
(Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations (pp.
3–90). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Kraimer, M. L., Takeuchi, R., & Frese, M. (2014). The global context and
people at work. Personnel Psychology, 67, 5–21. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/peps.12067
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Lam, S. S. K., Hui, C., & Law, K. S. (1999). Organizational citizenship
behavior: Comparing perspectives of supervisors and subordinates
across four international samples. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,
594 601. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.4.594
Lefkowitz, J. (2003). Ethics and values in industrial-organizational psy-
chology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leung, K. (1989). Cross-cultural differences: Individual-level vs. culture-
level analysis. International Journal of Psychology, 24, 703–719. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207598908247840
Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model for social beliefs
in multicultural perspective. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
36, 119–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(04)36003-X
Leung, K., & Morris, M. W. (2015). Values, schemas, and norms in the
culture– behavior nexus: A situated dynamics framework. Journal of
International Business Studies, 46, 1028 –1050. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1057/jibs.2014.66
Lisak, A., & Erez, M. (2015). Leadership emergence in multicultural
teams: The power of global characteristics. Journal of World Business,
50, 3–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2014.01.002
Liu, L. A., Chua, C. H., & Stahl, G. K. (2010). Quality of communication
experience: Definition, measurement, and implications for intercultural
negotiations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 469 487. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0019094
Lonner, W. J., & Adams, H. L. (1972). Interest patterns of psychologists in
nine Western nations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 146–151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0032656
Lowenstein, J., & Mueller, J. (2016). Implicit theories of creative Ideas:
How culture guides creativity assessments. Academy of Management
Discoveries, 2, 320 –348. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amd.2014.0147
Luiz, J. M. (2015). The impact of ethno-linguistic fractionalization on
cultural measures: Dynamics, endogeneity and modernization. Journal
of International Business Studies, 46, 1080–1098. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1057/jibs.2015.6
Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. (2014). Clash!: How to thrive in a multicul-
tural world. London, UK: Penguin.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications
for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224
253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
Marsden, D. (1991). Indigenous management. Journal of the History of
Economic Thought, 2, 21–38.
Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral research on Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk.
Behavior Research Methods, 44, 1–23. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0124-6
Matsui, T., & Terai, T. (1975). A cross-cultural study of the validity of the
expectancy theory of work motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
60, 263–265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076550
McClelland, D. C. (1961). Achieving society. New York, NY: Simon &
Schuster. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14359-000
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a
human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509 –516. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
526
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
Merrens, M. R., & Garrett, J. B. (1975). The Protestant Ethic Scale as a
predictor of repetitive work performance. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 60, 125–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076297
Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization
of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review,
1, 61– 89.
Minkov, M., Blagoev, V., & Bond, M. H. (2015). Improving research in
the emerging field of cross-cultural sociogenetics: The case of serotonin.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46, 336–354. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0022022114563612
Minkov, M., & Hofstede, G. (2014). Clustering of 316 European regions
on measures of values: Do Europe’s countries have national cultures?
Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science,
48, 144 –176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397113510866
Mohr, J. W., & Ghaziani, A. (2014). Problems and prospects of measure-
ment in the study of culture. Theory and Society, 43, 225–246. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11186-014-9227-2
Morgan, S. (2004). Positive risk-taking: An idea whose time has come.
Health Care Risk Report, 10, 18 –19.
Mrazek, A. J., Chiao, J. Y., Blizinsky, K. D., Lun, J., & Gelfand, M. J.
(2013). The role of culture–gene coevolution in morality judgment:
Examining the interplay between tightness–looseness and allelic varia-
tion of the serotonin transporter gene. Culture and Brain, 1, 100 –117.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40167-013-0009-x
Nouri, R., Erez, M., Lee, C., Liang, J., Bannister, B. D., & Chiu, W. (2015).
Social context: Key to understanding culture’s effects on creativity.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36, 899–918. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1002/job.1923
Nouri, R., Erez, M., Rockstuhl, T., Ang, S., Leshem-Calif, L., & Rafaeli,
A. (2013). Taking the bite out of culture: The impact of task structure
and task type on overcoming impediments to cross-cultural team per-
formance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34, 739 –763. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1002/job.1871
Nowak, A., Gelfand, M. J., Borkowski, W., Cohen, D., & Hernandez, I.
(2016). The evolutionary basis of honor cultures. Psychological Science,
27, 12–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797615602860
Oreg, S., Bayazit, M., Vakola, M., Arciniega, L., Armenakis, A.,
Barkauskiene, R.,...vanDam, K. (2008). Dispositional resistance to
change: Measurement equivalence and the link to personal values across
17 nations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 935–944. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.935
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. (2008). Does culture influence what and how
we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 134, 311–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134
.2.311
Peretz, H., & Fried, Y. (2012). National cultures, performance appraisal
practices, and organizational absenteeism and turnover: A study across
21 countries. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 448 459. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0026011
Plato, & Jowett, B. (1901). The republic of Plato; an ideal commonwealth
(Rev. ed.). New York, NY: Colonial Press.
Porat, A. M. (1970). Cross-cultural differences in resolving union-
management conflict through negotiations. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 54, 441– 451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029823
Pruitt, D. (2004). Forward. In M. J. Gelfand & J. M. Brett (Eds.), Hand-
book of negotiation and culture: Theoretical advances and cultural
perspectives (pp. xi–xiii). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
Qi, X. (2013). Guanxi, social capital theory and beyond: Toward a glo-
balized social science. British Journal of Sociology, 64, 308 –324. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-4446.12019
Ramesh, A., & Gelfand, M. J. (2010). Will they stay or will they go? The
role of job embeddedness in predicting turnover in individualistic and
collectivistic cultures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 807– 823.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019464
Raubenheimer, I. (1970). Influence of group and situational differences on
the applicability of a personnel test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54,
214 –216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029258
Roberts, K. H. (1970). On looking at an elephant. An evaluation of
cross-cultural research related to organizations. Psychological Bulletin,
74, 327–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0030140
Rockstuhl, T., Dulebohn, J. H., Ang, S., & Shore, L. M. (2012). Leader–
member exchange (LMX) and culture: A meta-analysis of correlates of
LMX across 23 countries. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 1097–
1130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029978
Rockstuhl, T., Seiler, S., Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Annen, H. (2011).
Beyond general intelligence (IQ) and emotional intelligence (EQ): The
role of cultural intelligence (CQ) on cross-border leadership effective-
ness in a globalized world. Journal of Social Issues, 67, 825– 840.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2011.01730.x
Ronen, S., & Shenkar, O. (2013). Mapping world cultures: Cluster forma-
tion, sources and implications. Journal of International Business Stud-
ies, 44, 867– 897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/jibs.2013.42
Roos, P., Gelfand, M., Nau, D., & Carr, R. (2014). High strength-of-ties
and low mobility enable the evolution of third-party punishment. Pro-
ceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 281,
20132661.
Roos, P., Gelfand, M., Nau, D., & Lun, J. (2015). Societal threat and
cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 14 –23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.01.003
Rousseau, D. M. (2000). Psychological contracts in the United States:
Associability, Individualism and Diversity. In D. M. Rousseau & R.
Schalk (Eds.), Psychological contracts in employment: Cross-national
perspectives (pp. 250 –282). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/
10.4135/9781452231273.n14
Salas, E., & Gelfand, M. J. (2013). Introduction to the Special Issue:
Collaboration in multicultural environments. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 34(6), 735–738.
Sampson, E. E. (1978). Scientific paradigms and social values: Wanted—A
scientific revolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36,
1332–1343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.36.11.1332
Sánchez, G. I. (1934). Bilingualism and mental measures: A word of
caution. Journal of Applied Psychology, 18, 765–772. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/h0072798
Sanchez-Burks, J. (2002). Protestant relational ideology and (in)attention
to relational cues in work settings. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 919–929. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.4
.919
Schaubroeck, J., Lam, S. S., & Xie, J. L. (2000). Collective efficacy versus
self-efficacy in coping responses to stressors and control: A cross-
cultural study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 512–525. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.85.4.512
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values:
Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1– 65).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The influence
of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.
Shaffer, M. A., Harrison, D. A., Gregersen, H., Black, J. S., & Ferzandi,
L. A. (2006). You can take it with you: individual differences and
expatriate effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 109 –125.
Shapira, Z., & Bass, B. M. (1975). Settling strikes in real life and simu-
lations in North America and different regions of Europe. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 60(4), 466 471.
Shokef, E., & Erez, M. (2008). Cultural intelligence and global identity in
multicultural teams. In S. Ang & L. V. Dyne (Eds.), Handbook of
cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications (pp. 177–
191). New York, NY: Routledge.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
527
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of
meaning. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Slocum, J. W., & Strawser, R. H. (1972). Racial differences in job atti-
tudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 28 –32.
Smith, P. B. (2008). Indigenous aspects of management. In P. B. Smith,
M. F. Peterson, & D. C. Thomas (Eds.), The handbook of cross-cultural
management research (pp. 319 –333). London, UK: Sage.
Smith, P. B., Huang, H. J., Harb, C., & Torres, C. (2012). How distinctive
are indigenous ways of achieving influence? A comparative study of
guanxi, wasta, jeitinho, and “pulling strings”. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 43, 135–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110381430
Stahl, G. K., & Caligiuri, P. (2005). The effectiveness of expatriate coping
strategies: the moderating role of cultural distance, position level, and
time on the international assignment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90,
603– 615.
Stahl, G. K., Maznevski, M. L., Voigt, A., & Jonsen, K. (2010). Unraveling
the effects of cultural diversity in teams: A meta-analysis of research on
multicultural work groups. Journal of International Business Studies,
41, 690 –709.
Steiner, D. D., & Gilliland, S. W. (1996). Fairness reactions to personnel
selection techniques in France and the United States. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 81, 134 –141.
Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., & Williams,
R. M. (1949). The American soldier: Adjustment during army life (Vol.
1). Oxford, England: Princeton University Press.
Stromquist, N. P. (2007). Internationalization as a response to globaliza-
tion: Radical shifts in university environments. Higher Education, 53,
81–105.
Sturman, M. C., Shao, L., & Katz, J. H. (2012). The effect of culture on the
curvilinear relationship between performance and turnover. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 97, 46 62.
Tannenbaum, A. S. (1980). Organizational psychology. In H. G. Triandis,
R. W. Brislin (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology (Vol. 5,
pp. 281–334). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Taras, V., Kirkman, B. L., & Steel, P. (2010). Examining the impact of
culture’s consequences: A three-decade, multilevel, meta-analytic re-
view of Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 95, 405– 439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018938
Taras, V., Steel, P., & Kirkman, B. L. (2016). Does country equal culture?
Beyond geography in the search for cultural boundaries. Management
International Review, 56, 455–487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11575-
016-0283-x
Taylor, F. W. (1914). The principles of scientific management. New York,
NY: Harper & Brothers.
te Nijenhuis, J., & van der Flier, H. (1997). Comparability of GATB scores
for immigrants and majority group members: Some Dutch findings.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 675– 687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0021-9010.82.5.675
Tinsley, C. (1998). Models of conflict resolution in Japanese, German, and
American cultures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 316 –323.
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York, NY:
Wiley.
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural
contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506 –520.
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture: Theoretical and methodological issues. In
H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of
industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 4). Palo Alto,
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Triandis, H. C., & Vassiliou, V. (1972). Interpersonal influence and em-
ployee selection in two cultures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56,
140 –145.
Tscheulin, D. (1973). Leader behavior measurement in German industry.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 28 –31.
Tsui, A. S., Nifadkar, S. S., & Ou, A. Y. (2007). Cross-national, cross-
cultural organizational behavior research: Advances, gaps, and recom-
mendations. Journal of Management, 33, 426 478. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0149206307300818
Tung, R. L., & Aycan, Z. (2008). Key success factors and indigenous
management practices in SMEs in emerging economies. Journal of
World Business, 43, 381–384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2008.04
.001
Tung, R. L., & Verbeke, A. (2010). Beyond Hofstede and GLOBE:
Improving the quality of cross-cultural research. Journal of International
Business Studies, 41, 1259 –1274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/jibs.2010.41
Tzu, S. (1963). The Art of war (S. B. Griffith, Trans.). New York, NY:
Oxford University.
Van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis of
comparative research. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Van de Vliert, E. (2013). Climato-economic habitats support patterns of
human needs, stresses, and freedoms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
36, 465– 480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002828
Venaik, S., & Midgley, D. F. (2015). Mindscapes across landscapes:
Archetypes of transnational and subnational culture. Journal of Interna-
tional Business Studies, 46, 1051–1079. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/jibs
.2015.11
Vignovic, J. A., & Thompson, L. F. (2010). Computer-mediated cross-
cultural collaboration: Attributing communication errors to the person
versus the situation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 265–276. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018628
Vinchur, A. J., & Koppes, L. L. (2011). A historical survey of research and
practice in industrial and organizational psychology. In S. Zedeck (Ed.),
APA handbook of industrial-organizational psychology (pp. 3–36).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/12169-001
Vygotsky, L. S., & Wollock, J. (1997). The collected works of LS Vygotsky:
Problems of the theory and history of psychology (Vol. 3). New York,
NY: Plenum Press.
Wasti, S. A., Bergman, M. E., Glomb, T. M., & Drasgow, F. (2000). Test
of the cross-cultural generalizability of a model of sexual harassment.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 766 –778. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0021-9010.85.5.766
Weber, M. (1958). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism (T. Parsons,
Trans.). New York, NY: Scribner. (Original work published 1905)
Whitehall, A. M., Jr. (1964). Cultural values and employee attitudes:
United States and Japan. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48, 69 –72.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040195
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. (1975). Children of six cultures: A
psycho-cultural analysis. Oxford, UK: Harvard University Press. http://
dx.doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674593770
Whiting, J. W., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality: A
cross-cultural study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Witkin, H. A., & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-
cultural perspective. ETS Research Bulletin Series, 1975 (1), i-100.
Wong, A., Tjosvold, D., & Yu, Z. Y. (2005). Organizational partnerships
in China: Self-interest, goal interdependence, and opportunism. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 90, 782–791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
9010.90.4.782
Worchel, S., & Mitchell, T. R. (1972). An evaluation of the effectiveness
of the culture assimilator in Thailand and Greece. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 56, 472– 479. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0033759
Wundt, W. (1906).
Völkerpsychologie, 1–3: Mythos und Religion. Leipzig,
Germany: Engelmann.
Yamagishi, T. (2011). Micro-macro dynamics of the cultural construction
of reality: A niche construction approach. In M. J. Gelfand, C.-Y. Chiu,
& Y. Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology (Vol. 1, pp.
251–308). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
528
GELFAND, AYCAN, EREZ, AND LEUNG
Yamawaki, N. (2012). Within-culture variations of collectivism in Japan.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43, 1191–1204. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0022022111428171
Zeidner, M. (1987). Test of the cultural bias hypothesis: Some Israeli
findings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 38 48. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0021-9010.72.1.38
Zellmer-Bruhn, M., & Gibson, C. B. (2014). How does culture matter? A
process view of cultural interaction in groups. In M. Yuki & M. Brewer
(Eds.), Frontiers of culture and psychology series: Culture and group
processes (pp. 166 –194). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Zhou, J., & Su, Y. (2010). A missing piece of the puzzle: The organiza-
tional context in cultural patterns of creativity. Management and Orga-
nization Review, 6, 391–413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-8784
.2010.00192.x
Zurcher, L. A. (1968). Particularism and organizational position: A cross-
cultural analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 139 –144. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025586
Received July 3, 2015
Revision received October 24, 2016
Accepted November 2, 2016
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
529
CROSS-CULTURAL I/O/OB